Results for ' lexical pragmatics'

965 found
Order:
  1.  75
    Lexical pragmatics and the geometry of opposition: The mystery of *nall and *nand revisited.Larry Horn - manuscript
    To appear in Jean-Yves Béziau (ed.) Proc. First World Congress on the Square of Opposition.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  2. A unitary approach to lexical pragmatics: relevance, inference and ad hoc concepts.Deirdre Wilson & Robyn Carston - 2007 - In Noel Burton-Roberts (ed.), Pragmatics. New York: Palgrave-Macmillan. pp. 3.
  3.  50
    Coevolution of Lexical Meaning and Pragmatic Use.Thomas Brochhagen, Michael Franke & Robert van Rooij - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (8):2757-2789.
    According to standard linguistic theory, the meaning of an utterance is the product of conventional semantic meaning and general pragmatic rules on language use. We investigate how such a division of labor between semantics and pragmatics could evolve under general processes of selection and learning. We present a game‐theoretic model of the competition between types of language users, each endowed with certain lexical representations and a particular pragmatic disposition to act on them. Our model traces two evolutionary forces (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  4. Lexical properties and pragmatic implications of some markers of temporal succession and simultaneity: A contrastive analysis of hungarian, norwegian, and English.Thorstein Fretheim Ildik Vask - 1996 - In Katarzyna Jaszczolt & Ken Turner (eds.), Contrastive semantics and pragmatics. Tarrytown, N.Y., U.S.A.: Pergamon Press. pp. 791-810.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  15
    Foundations of pragmatics and lexical semantics.Jeroen A. G. Groenendijk, Dick de Jongh & Martin J. B. Stokhof (eds.) - 1986 - Providence, RI, USA,: Foris Publications ;.
  6. Foundations of pragmatics and lexical semantics.M. B. J. Stokhof, Dick de Jongh & J. A. G. Groenendijk (eds.) - 1986 - Providence, RI, USA,: Foris Publications ;.
  7.  44
    A theory of if: A lexical entry, reasoning program, and pragmatic principles.Martin D. Braine & David P. O'Brien - 1991 - Psychological Review 98 (2):182-203.
  8.  7
    Identity of Linguistic Expressions and Lexical Synonymy in the Fields of Logical Semantics, Linguistic Semantics, and ‘Pragmatic Semantics’.Barbora Geistová Čakovská - 2011 - In Piotr Stalmaszczyk (ed.), Philosophical and Formal Approaches to Linguistic Analysis. Ontos. pp. 161-176.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  35
    Pragmatics.Noel Burton-Roberts (ed.) - 2007 - New York: Palgrave-Macmillan.
    This contribution to Palgrave's 'Advances' series addresses a wide range of issues that have arisen in post-Gricean pragmatic theory, in chapters by distinguished authors. Among the specific topics covered are scalar implicatures, lexical semantics and pragmatics, indexicality, procedural meaning, the semantics and pragmatics of negation. The volume includes both defences and critiques of Relevance Theory and of Neo-Gricean Pragmatics.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  10.  18
    Relevance Theory: Pragmatics and Cognition.Catherine Wearing - 2015 - WIREs Cognitive Science 6:87-95.
    Relevance Theory is a cognitively oriented theory of pragmatics, i.e., a theory of language use. It builds on the seminal work of H.P. Grice1 to develop a pragmatic theory which is at once philosophically sensitive and empirically plausible (in both psychological and evolutionary terms). This entry reviews the central commitments and chief contributions of Relevance Theory, including its Gricean commitment to the centrality of intention-reading and inference in communication; the cognitively grounded notion of relevance which provides the mechanism for (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  11.  42
    Pragmatic resolutions of temporal and aspectual mismatches.Louis de Saussure - 2021 - Pragmatics and Cognition 28 (2):228-251.
    This paper proposes a pragmatic solution to utterances where the various indicators of time and aspect (tenses, lexical-conceptual features of Aktionsart, adverb phrases and contextual cues) seem to have divergent temporal reference and aspectual properties. This type of cases is usually treated at the semantic level as ‘mismatches’ and resolved compositionally through logical operations of ‘aspectual coercion’. We suggest on the contrary that no such effect of ‘mismatch resolution’ or ‘coercion’ is at work: these utterances are worked out inferentially (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12. The Problem of Lexical Innovation.Josh Armstrong - 2016 - Linguistics and Philosophy 39 (2):87-118.
    In a series of papers, Donald Davidson :3–17, 1984, The philosophical grounds of rationality, 1986, Midwest Stud Philos 16:1–12, 1991) developed a powerful argument against the claim that linguistic conventions provide any explanatory purchase on an account of linguistic meaning and communication. This argument, as I shall develop it, turns on cases of what I call lexical innovation: cases in which a speaker uses a sentence containing a novel expression-meaning pair, but nevertheless successfully communicates her intended meaning to her (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  13.  3
    Exploring lexical associations in English as a Lingua Franca interactions.Yang Pang - 2024 - Pragmatics and Society 15 (5):779-799.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  42
    Pragmatics and Cognition: Intentions and Pattern Recognition in Context.Marco Mazzone - 2009 - International Review of Pragmatics 1 (2):321-347.
    The importance of intention reading for communication has already been emphasized many<br>years ago by Paul Grice. More recently, the rich debate on “theory of mind” has convinced many<br>that intention reading may in fact play a key role also in current, cognitively oriented theories of<br>pragmatics: Relevance Th eory is a case in point. On a close analysis, however, it is far from clear<br>that RT may really accommodate the idea that intention reading drives comprehension. Here<br>I examine RT’s diffi culties with that (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  15.  18
    On Encoded Lexical Meaning: Philosophical and Psychological Perspectives.Stavros Assimakopoulos - 2012 - Humana Mente 5 (23).
    The past few years have seen quite a bit of speculation over relevance theorists’ commitment to Fodorian semantics as a means to account for the notion of encoded lexical meaning that they put forth in their framework. In this paper, I take on the issue, arguing that this view of lexical semantics compromises Relevance Theory’s aim of psychological plausibility, since it effectively binds it with the ‘literal first’ hypothesis that has been deemed unrealistic from a psycholinguistic viewpoint. After (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  16. A trilemma for the lexical utility model of the precautionary principle.H. Orri Stefánsson - 2024 - Philosophical Studies 181 (12):3271-3287.
    Bartha and DesRoches (Synthese 199(3–4):8701–8740, 2021) and Steel and Bartha (Risk Analysis 43(2):260–268, 2023) argue that we should understand the precautionary principle as the injunction to maximise lexical utilities. They show that the lexical utility model has important pragmatic advantages. Moreover, the model has the theoretical advantage of satisfying all axioms of expected utility theory except continuity. In this paper I raise a trilemma for any attempt at modelling the precautionary principle with lexical utilities: it permits choice (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  17.  17
    Semantics and Pragmatics: Drawing a Line.Raphael Salkie & Ilse Depraetere (eds.) - 2016 - Cham, Switzerland: Springer Verlag.
    This book explores new territory at the interface between semantics and pragmatics, reassessing a number of linguistic phenomena in the light of recent advances in pragmatic theory and presenting stimulating insights by experts in linguistics and philosophy. The authors begin by reassessing the definition of four theoretical concepts: saturation, free pragmatic enrichment, completion and expansion. They go on to confront disciplines that have addressed similar issues but that have not necessarily been in close contact, and then turn to questions (...)
    No categories
  18.  26
    Cognitive constraints in English lexical blending.Daniel Kjellander - 2018 - Pragmatics and Cognition 25 (1):142-173.
    The complex characteristics of lexical blending have long troubled mainstream word formation research to the extent that it has typically been considered a peripheral issue in linguistics. In recent years this has begun to change, and there is currently a growing body of evidence uncovering the intriguing nature of this word formation process. In the present study, underlying principles and usage-based aspects of lexical blends were examined. Analyses of derivatives of three matrix words,republican, liberal, andvegetarian, revealed the impact (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  19. Pragmatic Enrichment.Francois Recanati - 2011 - In Gillian Russell & Delia Graff Fara (eds.), Routledge Companion to Philosophy of Language. New York, USA: Routledge. pp. 67-78.
    It is commonly held that all truth-conditional effects of context result from a pragmatic process of value-assignment that is triggered (and made obligatory) by something in the sentence itself, namely a lexically context-sensitive expression (e.g. an indexical) or a free variable in logical form. Such a process has been dubbed ‘saturation'. It stands in contrast to so called ‘free' pragmatic processes, which are supposed to take place for purely pragmatic reasons — in order to make sense of what the speaker (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  20.  14
    Pragmatic Ambiguity: The Thetic Function of Modality.Paolo Di Lucia & Amedeo Conte - 2009 - Lodz Papers in Pragmatics 5 (2):191-199.
    Pragmatic Ambiguity: The Thetic Function of Modality The aim of this paper is to present an overview of the pragmatic aspects of ambiguity present in deontic sentences, which may have three pragmatic functions: a prescriptive or a descriptive or a constitutive function. This type of ambiguity is investigated on the lexical, phrasal, and sentential level. The discussion focuses on the deontic constructions of the German verb sollen and the English shall as they are used in legal texts. It also (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  21.  40
    The pragmatic view on dual character concepts and expressions.Lucien Baumgartner - 2024 - Mind and Language 39 (5):726-744.
    This article introduces a new pragmatic framework for dual character concepts and their expressions, offering an alternative to the received lexical‐semantic view. On the prevalent lexical‐semantic view, expressions such as “philosopher” or “scientist” are construed as lexical polysemes, comprising both a descriptive and a normative dimension. Thereby, this view prioritizes established norms, neglecting normative expressions emerging in specific contexts. In contrast, the pragmatic view integrates pragmatic modulation as a central element in explaining context‐dependent dual character concepts and (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  74
    Non-lexical conversational sounds in American English.Nigel Ward - 2006 - Pragmatics and Cognition 14 (1):129-182.
    Sounds like h-nmm, hh-aaaah, hn-hn, unkay, nyeah, ummum, uuh, um-hm-uh-hm, um and uh-huh occur frequently in American English conversation but have thus far escaped systematic study. This article reports a study of both the forms and functions of such tokens in a corpus of American English conversations. These sounds appear not to be lexical, in that they are productively generated rather than finite in number, and in that the sound–meaning mapping is compositional rather than arbitrary. This implies that English (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  23.  20
    Pragmatically pristine, the dialogical cause of self-deception.Colin T. Schmidt - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (1):126-126.
    Empirical evidence of self-deception's propositional duality is not sought; philosophically relevant links between propositions proper and mind are explored instead. Speech in unison ably indicates the social grounding of such attitudinal structures. An extra-theoretical eye – with regard to cognitivism – is cast on a case of “illusory communication.” The reinforcing of lexical analysis shows Mele's approach to be in need of non-ego concepts, wherefore it lacks soundness with respect to reference.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  24. From etymology to pragmatics: metaphorical and cultural aspects of semantic structure.Eve Sweetser - 1990 - New York: Cambridge University Press.
    This book offers a new approach to the analysis of the multiple meanings of English modals, conjunctions, conditionals, and perception verbs. Although such ambiguities cannot easily be accounted for by feature-analyses of word meaning, Eve Sweetser's argument shows that they can be analyzed both readily and systematically. Meaning relationships in general cannot be understood independently of human cognitive structure, including the metaphorical and cultural aspects of that structure. Sweetser shows that both lexical polysemy and pragmatic ambiguity are shaped by (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   127 citations  
  25. Challenges to Bach’s Pragmatics.Esther Romero & Belén Soria - 2013 - Croatian Journal of Philosophy 13 (2):135-160.
    In this paper, we will revise Bach’s classification of contents in what is directly meant. That catalogue was introduced to reach an exhaustive characterization of the contents that may appear in what the speaker means; something that cannot be done just with Grice’s division between what is said and what is implied. However, Bach’s distinction among different types of direct inexplicit contents presents some theoretical problems which we think can be avoided if at least the following is considered. First, within (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  30
    Lexical change often begins and ends in semantic peripheries.Susanne Vejdemo - 2018 - Pragmatics and Cognition 25 (1):50-85.
    The article discusses semantic change and lexical replacement processes in the color domain, based on color naming studies in seven Germanic languages and from different generations of speakers in a single language. Change in the color domain often begins and ends in conceptual peripheries, and I argue that this perspective is suitable for other semantic domains as well.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  27.  31
    Metonymy triggers syntactic argument alternation: vehicle for conductor metonymy as a constraint on lexical-constructional integration.Luana Amaral & Márcia Cançado - 2020 - Cognitive Linguistics 31 (1):113-148.
    This paper explores the role of metonymy in determining a syntactic argument alternation (“conductor-vehiclealternation”) which occurs in English and Portuguese:o piloto acelerou a Ferrari“the driver speeded up the Ferrari”/a Ferrari acelerou“the Ferrari speeded up/sped away”. Since the verbs in theconductor-vehiclealternation haveconductorandvehiclearguments (controller and controlled entities), a metonymic process can occur, allowing thevehicleexpression to provide access to theconductorparticipant. To explain how metonymy allows a verb with two participants to be integrated into a construction with a single argument, we assume that metonymy (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28. Towards a type-theoretical account of lexical semantics.Christian Bassac, Bruno Mery & Christian Retoré - 2010 - Journal of Logic, Language and Information 19 (2):229-245.
    After a quick overview of the field of study known as “Lexical Semantics”, where we advocate the need of accessing additional information besides syntax and Montague-style semantics at the lexical level in order to complete the full analysis of an utterance, we summarize the current formulations of a well-known theory of that field. We then propose and justify our own model of the Generative Lexicon Theory, based upon a variation of classical compositional semantics, and outline its formalization. Additionally, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  29.  15
    Pragmatic markers and discourse coherence relations in English and Catalan oral narrative.Montserrat González - 2005 - Discourse Studies 7 (1):53-86.
    This article explores the role that markers play in the pragmatic discourse structure of Catalan and English oral narratives. It is argued that their meaning is directly related to the sort of coherence relation that they establish with preceding and following propositions and discourse segments, centring the discussion on four discourse structures/components: ideational, rhetorical, sequential and inferential. The aim is to show the textual form-pragmatic function relationship by means of specific lexical units placed at specific parts of the narrative. (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  30.  11
    Pragmatics and the Lexicon.Laurence Horn - 2016 - In Yan Huang (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Pragmatics. Oxford University Press UK.
    Since Paul and Zipf, it has become evident that lexical choice and meaning change are largely guided by pragmatic principles. Two central interacting principles are, first, the least-effort tendency to reduce expression and, second, the communicative requirements on sufficiency of information. Descendants of this opposition include Grice’s bipartite Maxim of Quantity grounded within a general theory of rationality and cooperation, the Q and R Principles, and the interplay of effort and effect within Relevance Theory. This chapter motivates a constraint (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  21
    Interactive Alignment and Lexical Triggering of Code-Switching in Bilingual Dialogue.Gerrit Jan Kootstra, Ton Dijkstra & Janet G. van Hell - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11:539420.
    When bilingual speakers use two languages in the same utterance, this is called code-switching. Previous research indicates that bilinguals’ likelihood to code-switch is enhanced when the utterance to be produced (1) contains a word with a similar form across languages (lexical triggering) and (2) is preceded by a code-switched utterance, for example from a dialogue partner (interactive alignment/priming of code-switching). Both factors have mostly been tested on corpus data and have not yet been studied in combination. In two experiments, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  20
    Contraconscious Internal Theories Influence Lexical Choice during Sentence Completion.Donald G. MacKay & Toshi Konishi - 1994 - Consciousness and Cognition 3 (2):196-222.
    This paper examines how inner theories influence lexical choice. Subjects in one study completed auditorily presented sentence fragments, some of which contained nonhuman antecedents such as dog and cat. Subjects were more likely to use human pronouns rather than it for referring to pets rather than nonpets , and antecedents that were liked rather than disliked , familiar rather than unfamiliar , named rather than unnamed , rational rather than nonrational , and engaging in typically human rather than nonhuman (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  59
    The pragmatic marker like in English teen talk.Farzad Sharifian & Ian G. Malcolm - 2003 - Pragmatics and Cognition 11 (2):327-344.
    This study reports on the use of like in Aboriginal English teen talk. The analysis of a sub-corpus of 40 adolescent texts from a corpus of 100 narratives by speakers of Aboriginal English in Western Australia revealed that like is often employed by these speakers, and that it achieves a multitude of functions. In general it is observed that like may mark off a) a discrepancy between the intended conceptualization and the expressed concept, b) an attitude, feeling, or certain degree (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34. The Time-Course of Sentence Meaning Composition. N400 Effects of the Interaction between Context-Induced and Lexically Stored Affordances.Erica Cosentino, Giosuè Baggio, Jarmo Kontinen & Markus Werning - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8:248173.
    Contemporary semantic theories can be classified along two dimensions: (i) the way and time-course in which contextual factors influence sentence truth-conditions; and (ii) whether and to what extent comprehension involves sensory, motor and emotional processes. In order to explore this theoretical space, our ERP study investigates the time-course of the interaction between the lexically specified telic component of a noun (the function of the object to which the noun refers to, e.g., a funnel is generally used to pour liquids into (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  35. On the semantics/pragmatics distinction.Brendan S. Gillon - 2008 - Synthese 165 (3):373-384.
    This paper addresses two questions: what is the distinction between semantics and pragmatics? And why is this distinction important? These questions are discussed in light of the central explanatory goal of linguistics and in relation to the phenomenon of context sensitivity, as illustrated by relational words with implicit arguments and by so-called quantifier domain restriction. It is concluded that context sensitivity is, in the former case, grammatical or lexical and, in the latter case, neither.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  36. Prepragmatics: Widening the Semantics-Pragmatics Boundary.Isidora Stojanovic - 2014 - In Alexis Burgess & Brett Sherman (eds.), Metasemantics: New Essays on the Foundations of Meaning. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 311-326.
    One of the most important and, at the same time, most controversial issues in metasemantics is the question of what semantics is, and what distinguishes semantic elements (features, properties, phenomena, mechanisms, processes, or whatever) from the rest. The issue is tightly linked with the debate over the semantics-pragmatics distinction, which has been vibrant for a decade or two, but seems to be reaching an impasse. I suggest that this impasse may be due to the failure to recognize a distinct (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  37.  35
    The dynamics of lexical innovation.Quirin Würschinger, Jelena Prokić, Daphné Kerremans & Hans-Jörg Schmid - 2018 - Pragmatics and Cognition 25 (1):1-7.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  42
    Nanti self-quotation: Implications for the pragmatics of reported speech and evidentiality.Lev Michael - 2012 - Pragmatics and Society 3 (2):321-357.
    This paper describes two quotation strategies employed by speakers of Nanti, one involving grammaticalized quotatives and another involving complement-taking verbs of saying, and examines the consequences of the pragmatic differences between these strategies for two key questions in the study of evidentiality: first, the importance of degree of grammaticalization in delimiting ‘evidentials’; and second, the importance of the analytical distinction between epistemic modal and ‘source of information’ evidential meanings. Nanti use of the two quotation strategies is specifically analyzed in the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  39.  25
    Referring to parents in child protection reporting: A pragmatic-discursive study of a sensitive issue.Marie Veniard - 2011 - Pragmatics and Society 2 (2):301-327.
    This paper considers the terms used for designating parents in reports dealing with child protection, and explores the pragmatic impact of the reports’ extremely cautious choice of words. I test the hypothesis that, even if words are not argumentative in themselves, they can become argumentative in the context of a particular discourse. To this end, this paper develops a two-pronged analysis, combining lexical description with quantitative as well as qualitative methodologies. The findings suggest that lexis is argumentative not only (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  42
    Using data-mining to identify and study patterns in lexical innovation on the web.Daphné Kerremans, Jelena Prokić, Quirin Würschinger & Hans-Jörg Schmid - 2018 - Pragmatics and Cognition 25 (1):174-200.
    This paper presents the NeoCrawler – a tailor-made webcrawler, which identifies and retrieves neologisms from the Internet and systematically monitors the use of detected neologisms on the web by means of weekly searches. It enables researchers to use the web as a corpus in order to investigate the dynamics of lexical innovation on a large-scale and systematic basis. The NeoCrawler represents an innovative web-mining tool which opens up new opportunities for linguists to tackle a number of unresolved and under-researched (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41. The Syntax and Pragmatics of The Naming Relation.Kenneth A. Taylor - unknown
    Philosophers of language have lavished attention on names and other singular referring expressions. But they have focused primarily on what might be called lexicalsemantic character of names and have largely ignored both what I call the lexicalsyntactic character of names and also what I call the pragmatic significance of the naming relation. Partly as a consequence, explanatory burdens have mistakenly been heaped upon semantics that properly belong elsewhere. This essay takes some steps toward correcting these twin lacunae. When we properly (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  42.  17
    On metonymy-based lexical innovations in Nigerian Pidgin English and Tok Pisin: A cognitive linguistic perspective.Krzysztof Kosecki - 2023 - Lodz Papers in Pragmatics 19 (1):49-70.
    As contact languages, pidgins and creoles arise in mixed linguistic environments. Drawing much of their vocabularies from one, frequently European, language and – to a lesser extent – from a number of indigenous languages, they have lexicons that are reduced in comparison with those of their lexifiers. To compensate for the poor lexification, pidgin and creoles create novel polysemy-based extensions of lexical items or develop periphrastic constructions equivalent of the missing lexical roots. Assuming a cognitive linguistic perspective, which (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  31
    From speaker innovation to lexical change.Terttu Nevalainen - 2018 - Pragmatics and Cognition 25 (1):8-29.
    Applying a sociolinguistic approach to the study of neologisms, this paper discusses the actuation and diffusion of new words in Early Modern English (EModE; 1500–1700) and draws some parallels with word coining in the comparable but more recent period of Early Modern Finnish (EModF; 1810–1880). The success of this exercise ultimately depends on the data and tools available for ascertaining the status of neologisms in a broader synchronic and diachronic context. The use of historical dictionaries and digital databases shows that (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  2
    Deviance and the literal-metaphorical distinction revisited.Chris Genovesi & Jacob Hesse - 2025 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy:1-25.
    Classical deviance theories about metaphor argue that the metaphorical sense of a word or expression, w, deviates from the sense of the word or expression interpreted literally. Developments in lexical pragmatics challenge these theories by claiming that deviance pervades (nearly) all aspects of linguistic communication. If deviance is the norm, then classical explanans offer little to no insight. In fact, many theorists have abandoned the idea of the literal-metaphorical distinction. This move carries significant consequences for theories of language (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45. What words mean and express: semantics and pragmatics of kind terms and verbs.Agustin Vicente - 2017 - Journal of Pragmatics 117:231-244.
    For many years, it has been common-ground in semantics and in philosophy of language that semantics is in the business of providing a full explanation about how propositional meanings are obtained. This orthodox picture seems to be in trouble these days, as an increasing number of authors now hold that semantics does not deal with thought-contents. Some of these authors have embraced a “thin meanings” view, according to which lexical meanings are too schematic to enter propositional contents. I will (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  36
    Using corpus methodology for semantic and pragmatic analyses: What can corpora tell us about the linguistic expression of emotions?Ulrike Oster - 2010 - Cognitive Linguistics 21 (4):727-763.
    The aim of this paper is to explore some of the possibilities, advantages and difficulties of corpus-based analyses of semantic and pragmatic aspects of language in one particular field, namely the linguistic expression of emotion concepts. For this purpose, a methodological procedure is proposed and an exemplary analysis of the emotion concept “fear” in English is performed. The procedure combines Kövecses' lexical approach and Stefanowitsch's metaphorical pattern analysis with additional concepts from corpus linguistics such as semantic preference and semantic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  47. Meaning, modulation, and context: a multidimensional semantics for truth-conditional pragmatics.Guillermo Del Pinal - 2018 - Linguistics and Philosophy 41 (2):165-207.
    The meaning that expressions take on particular occasions often depends on the context in ways which seem to transcend its direct effect on context-sensitive parameters. ‘Truth-conditional pragmatics’ is the project of trying to model such semantic flexibility within a compositional truth-conditional framework. Most proposals proceed by radically ‘freeing up’ the compositional operations of language. I argue, however, that the resulting theories are too unconstrained, and predict flexibility in cases where it is not observed. These accounts fall into this position (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  48.  39
    When Is a Metaphor Not a Metaphor? An Investigation Into Lexical Characteristics of Metaphoricity Among Uncertain Cases.Katie J. Patterson - 2017 - Metaphor and Symbol 32 (2):103-117.
    This article explores the ways in which language users make sense of metaphoricity when manifest in a variety of ways within the language. The research provides an analysis of the lexical characteristics of a single item when used in potentially, but not clearly identified, metaphoric contexts. The analysis focuses on flexible patterns of meaning and the relationship between metaphor and other aspects of figurative language such as polysemy, metonymy, and meronymy. The research stands as a follow up to a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  49.  8
    Scalarity of the Japanese initial mora-based minimizer: a compositional (lexically unspecified) minimizer and a non-compositional (lexically specified) minimizer.Osamu Sawada - 2023 - Natural Language Semantics 31 (2):71-120.
    This study investigates interpretations of the Japanese initial mora-based minimizer “X.Y...”-_no_ “X”-_no ji-mo_ ‘lit. even the letter “X” of “X.Y...”.’ Although initial mora-based minimizers have a literal interpretation of _ji_ ‘letter’, they have a non-literal interpretation as well. The non-literal interpretation has several distinctive features that are not present in ordinary minimizers. First, it is highly productive in that various expressions can appear in the form “X.Y...”-_no_ “X”-_no ji_. Second, non-literal minimizers typically co-occur with predicates that relate to knowledge, information, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  89
    (1 other version)Memes as multimodal metaphors.Kate Scott - 2021 - Pragmatics and Cognition 28 (2):277-298.
    In this article I analyse object labelling image macro internet memes as multimodal metaphors, taking the Distracted Boyfriend meme as a case study. Object labelling memes are multimodal texts in which users add labels to a stock photograph to convey messages that are often humorous or satirical in nature. Using the relevance-theoretic account of metaphor, I argue that object labelling memes are multimodal metaphors which are interpreted using the same processes as verbal metaphors. The labelling of the image guides the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 965