Results for 'Visual Reference'

971 found
Order:
  1. Visual Reference and Iconic Content.Santiago Echeverri - 2017 - Philosophy of Science 84 (4):761-781.
    Evidence from cognitive science supports the claim that humans and other animals see the world as divided into objects. Although this claim is widely accepted, it remains unclear whether the mechanisms of visual reference have representational content or are directly instantiated in the functional architecture. I put forward a version of the former approach that construes object files as icons for objects. This view is consistent with the evidence that motivates the architectural account, can respond to the key (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  2. Seeing and Visual Reference.Kevin J. Lande - 2021 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research (2):402-433.
    Perception is a central means by which we come to represent and be aware of particulars in the world. I argue that an adequate account of perception must distinguish between what one perceives and what one's perceptual experience is of or about. Through capacities for visual completion, one can be visually aware of particular parts of a scene that one nevertheless does not see. Seeing corresponds to a basic, but not exhaustive, way in which one can be visually aware (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  3.  96
    Attentive Visual Reference.E. J. Green - 2017 - Mind and Language 32 (1):3-38.
    Many have held that when a person visually attends to an object, her visual system deploys a representation that designates the object. Call the referential link between such representations and the objects they designate attentive visual reference. In this article I offer an account of attentive visual reference. I argue that the object representations deployed in visual attention—which I call attentive visual object representations —refer directly, and are akin to indexicals. Then I turn (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  4. Does visual reference depend on sortal classification? Reply to Clark.John Campbell - 2006 - Philosophical Studies 127 (2):221-237.
  5.  71
    Bioethics Resources on the Web.National Reference Center for Bioethics Literature - 2000 - Kennedy Institute of Ethics Journal 10 (2):175-188.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Kennedy Institute of Ethics Journal 10.2 (2000) 175-188 [Access article in PDF] Scope Note 38 Bioethics Resources on the Web * Once described as an "enormous used book store with volumes stacked on shelves and tables and overflowing onto the floor" (Pool, Robert. 1994. Turning an Info-Glut into a Library. Science 266 (7 October): 20-22, p. 20), Internet resources now receive numerous levels of organization, from basic directory listings (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6.  24
    Inferring the visual reference.Fred H. Previc - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):324-325.
  7.  14
    On Visually-Grounded Reference Production: Testing the Effects of Perceptual Grouping and 2D/3D Presentation Mode.Ruud Koolen - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:470418.
    When referring to a target object in a visual scene, speakers are assumed to consider certain distractor objects to be more relevant than others. The current research predicts that the way in which speakers come to a set of relevant distractors depends on how they perceive the distance between the objects in the scene. It reports on the results of two language production experiments, in which participants referred to target objects in photo-realistic visual scenes. Experiment 1 manipulated three (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  8.  58
    Visual Complexity and Its Effects on Referring Expression Generation.Micha Elsner, Alasdair Clarke & Hannah Rohde - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S4):940-973.
    Speakers’ perception of a visual scene influences the language they use to describe it—which objects they choose to mention and how they characterize the relationships between them. We show that visual complexity can either delay or facilitate description generation, depending on how much disambiguating information is required and how useful the scene's complexity can be in providing, for example, helpful landmarks. To do so, we measure speech onset times, eye gaze, and utterance content in a reference production (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  9. On visual experience of objects: Comments on John Campbell's reference and consciousness.Mohan Matthen - 2006 - Philosophical Studies 127 (2):195-220.
    John Campbell argues that visual attention to objects is the means by which we can refer to objects, and that this is so because conscious visual attention enables us to retrieve information about a location. It is argued here that while Campbell is right to think that we visually attend to objects, he does not give us sufficient ground for thinking that consciousness is involved, and is wrong to assign an intermediary role to location. Campbell’s view on sortals (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  10.  14
    Referent Cueing, Position, and Animacy as Accessibility Factors in Visually Situated Sentence Production.Yulia Esaulova, Martina Penke & Sarah Dolscheid - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11:518803.
    Speakers’ readiness to describe event scenes using active or passive constructions has previously been attributed—among other factors—to the accessibility of referents. While most research has highlighted the accessibility of agents, the present study examines whether patients’ accessibility can be modulated by means of visual preview of the patient character (derived accessibility), as well as by manipulating the animacy status of patients (inherent accessibility). Crucially, we also examined whether effects of accessibility were amenable to the visuospatial position of the patient (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  11. Visual Attention Fixes Demonstrative Reference By Eliminating Referential Luck.Imogen Dickie - 2011 - In Christopher Mole, Declan Smithies & Wayne Wu, Attention: Philosophical and Psychological Essays. New York: Oxford University Press.
  12. Reference and Consciousness.John Campbell - 2002 - Oxford, GB: Oxford University Press.
    John Campbell investigates how consciousness of the world explains our ability to think about the world; how our ability to think about objects we can see depends on our capacity for conscious visual attention to those things. He illuminates classical problems about thought, reference, and experience by looking at the underlying psychological mechanisms on which conscious attention depends.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   673 citations  
  13.  23
    Mapping visual spatial prototypes: Multiple reference frames shape visual memory.Elena Azañón, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Metodi Siromahov, Elena Amoruso & Matthew R. Longo - 2020 - Cognition 198 (C):104199.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  14.  34
    Early visual deprivation prompts the use of body-centered frames of reference for auditory localization.Tiziana Vercillo, Alessia Tonelli & Monica Gori - 2018 - Cognition 170 (C):263-269.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  15.  27
    Making Referents Seen and Heard Across Signed and Spoken Languages: Documenting and Interpreting Cross-Modal Differences in the Use of Enactment.Sébastien Vandenitte - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13:784339.
    Differences in language use and structures between signed and spoken languages have often been attributed to so-called language “modality.” Indeed, this is derived from the conception that spoken languages resort to both the oral-aural channel of speech and the visual-kinesic channel of visible bodily action whereas signed languages only resort to the latter. This paper addresses the use of enactment, a depictive communicative strategy whereby language users imitate referents in signed and spoken languages. Reviewing comparative research on enactment, this (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16. Visual receptive field organisation and spatial reference transformation in macaque posterior parietal cortex.V. Prevosto, F. Klam & W. Graf - 1996 - In Enrique Villanueva, Perception. Ridgeview Pub. Co. pp. 166-166.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  14
    Visual inhomogeneity and reference frames.Miguel A. GarcÍa-PÉrez - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (1):21-24.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18. Visual indexes and nonconceptual reference.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - manuscript
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19. Visual–Auditory fusion and frames of reference.C. Roumes, D. Hartnagel & M. Godfroy - 1996 - In Enrique Villanueva, Perception. Ridgeview Pub. Co. pp. 33.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  29
    Does the reference signal cancel visual field motion?Arnold E. Stoper - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):333-334.
  21.  58
    Reference Production as Search: The Impact of Domain Size on the Production of Distinguishing Descriptions.Gatt Albert, Krahmer Emiel, van Deemter Kees & P. G. van Gompel Roger - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S6):1459-1492.
    When producing a description of a target referent in a visual context, speakers need to choose a set of properties that distinguish it from its distractors. Computational models of language production/generation usually model this as a search process and predict that the time taken will increase both with the number of distractors in a scene and with the number of properties required to distinguish the target. These predictions are reminiscent of classic findings in visual search; however, unlike models (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  22.  41
    Modeling Reference Production as the Probabilistic Combination of Multiple Perspectives.Mindaugas Mozuraitis, Suzanne Stevenson & Daphna Heller - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S4):974-1008.
    While speakers have been shown to adapt to the knowledge state of their addressee in choosing referring expressions, they often also show some egocentric tendencies. The current paper aims to provide an explanation for this “mixed” behavior by presenting a model that derives such patterns from the probabilistic combination of both the speaker's and the addressee's perspectives. To test our model, we conducted a language production experiment, in which participants had to refer to objects in a context that also included (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  23.  75
    The Dynamics of Reference and Shared Visual Attention.Rick Dale, Natasha Z. Kirkham & Daniel C. Richardson - 2011 - Frontiers in Psychology 2.
  24.  23
    Frames of reference in unilateral neglect and visual perception: A computational perspective.Michael C. Mozer - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (1):156-185.
  25.  11
    Realistic About Reference Production: Testing the Effects of Domain Size and Saturation.Ruud Koolen & Emiel Krahmer - 2024 - Cognitive Science 48 (6):e13473.
    Experiments on visually grounded, definite reference production often manipulate simple visual scenes in the form of grids filled with objects, for example, to test how speakers are affected by the number of objects that are visible. Regarding the latter, it was found that speech onset times increase along with domain size, at least when speakers refer to nonsalient target objects that do not pop out of the visual domain. This finding suggests that even in the case of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  27
    Frames of reference for the light-from-above prior in visual search and shape judgements.Wendy J. Adams - 2008 - Cognition 107 (1):137-150.
  27. Reference, perception, and attention.Athanasios Raftopoulos - 2009 - Philosophical Studies 144 (3):339 - 360.
    I examine John Campbell’s claim that the determination of the reference of a perceptual demonstrative requires conscious visual object-based selective attention. I argue that although Campbell’s claim to the effect that, first, a complex binding parameter is needed to establish the referent of a perceptual demonstrative, and, second, that this referent is determined independently of, and before, the application of sortals is correct, this binding parameter does not require object-based attention for its construction. If object-based attention were indeed (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  28.  32
    Separability of reference frame distinctions from motor and visual images.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-225.
  29.  24
    The Historical Roots of Visual Analog Scale in Psychology as Revealed by Reference Publication Year Spectroscopy.Andy Wai Kan Yeung & Natalie Sui Miu Wong - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  30. Implication of head centric reference in visual perception.M. Guerraz, D. Poquin & T. Ohlmann - 1996 - In Enrique Villanueva, Perception. Ridgeview Pub. Co. pp. 137-137.
  31.  37
    Positional priming of visual pop-out search is supported by multiple spatial reference frames.Ahu Gokce, Hermann J. Müller & Thomas Geyer - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32. Nonconceptual demonstrative reference.Athanassius Raftopoulos & Vincent Muller - 2006 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 72 (2):251-285.
    The paper argues that the reference of perceptual demonstratives is fixed in a causal nondescriptive way through the nonconceptual content of perception. That content consists first in spatiotemporal information establishing the existence of a separate persistent object retrieved from a visual scene by the perceptual object segmentation processes that open an object-file for that object. Nonconceptual content also consists in other transducable information, that is, information that is retrieved directly in a bottom-up way from the scene (motion, shape, (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  33.  13
    Incidental encoding of visual information in temporal reference frames in working memory.Anna Heuer & Martin Rolfs - 2021 - Cognition 207 (C):104526.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  31
    EEG can Track the Time Course of Successful Reference Resolution in Small Visual Worlds.Christian Brodbeck, Laura Gwilliams & Liina Pylkkänen - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:1787.
    Previous research has shown that language comprehenders resolve reference quickly and incrementally, but not much is known about the neural processes and representations that are involved. Studies of visual short-term memory suggest that access to the representation of an item from a previously seen display is associated with a negative evoked potential at posterior electrodes contralateral to the spatial location of that item in the display. In this paper we demonstrate that resolving the reference of a noun (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  35.  17
    Interpreting Estonian Demonstratives: The Effects of Referent’s Distance and Visual Salience.Maria Reile, Kristiina Averin & Nele Põldver - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Most of the research done with spatial demonstratives have focused on the production, not the interpretation, of these words. In addition, emphasis has been largely on demonstrative pronouns, leaving demonstrative adverbs with relatively little research attention. The present study explores the interpretation of both demonstrative pronouns and demonstrative adverbs in Estonian—a Finno-Ugric language with two dialectal-specific demonstrative pronoun systems. In the South-Estonian dialectal region, two demonstrative pronouns, see—“this” and too—“that”, are used. In the North-Estonian region, only one, see—“this/that”, is used. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  84
    Reference and Subjectivity.Bill Brewer - 2004 - In John Greco, Ernest Sosa: And His Critics. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 215–223.
    In ‘Fregean Reference Defended’ (1995), Sosa presents a sophisticated descriptive theory of reference, which he calls ‘fregean’, and which he argues avoids standard counterexamples to more basic variants of this approach. What is characteristic of a fregean theory, in his sense, is the idea that what makes a person’s thought about some object, a, a thought about that particular thing, is the fact that a uniquely satisfies an appropriate individuator which is suitably operative in her thinking.1 On his (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  37.  25
    No-Reference Stereoscopic Image Quality Assessment Based on Binocular Statistical Features and Machine Learning.Peng Xu, Man Guo, Lei Chen, Weifeng Hu, Qingshan Chen & Yujun Li - 2021 - Complexity 2021:1-14.
    Learning a deep structure representation for complex information networks is a vital research area, and assessing the quality of stereoscopic images or videos is challenging due to complex 3D quality factors. In this paper, we explore how to extract effective features to enhance the prediction accuracy of perceptual quality assessment. Inspired by the structure representation of the human visual system and the machine learning technique, we propose a no-reference quality assessment scheme for stereoscopic images. More specifically, the statistical (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  52
    Self-reference: Theory and didactics between language and literature.Svend Erik Larsen - 2005 - Journal of Aesthetic Education 39 (1):13-30.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Self-Reference:Theory and Didactics between Language and LiteratureSvend Erik Larsen (bio)Semiotics of Self-ReferenceLiterary metafiction constitutes the extreme case of self-referential texts. Therefore we can either discard it as generally irrelevant for the understanding of the cultural functions of texts, or use it as a point of departure for the formulation of both general and basic aspects of such functions. The position taken in this essay will opt for the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  53
    Color in Reference Production: The Role of Color Similarity and Color Codability.Jette Viethen, Thomas Vessem, Martijn Goudbeek & Emiel Krahmer - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S6):1493-1514.
    It has often been observed that color is a highly preferred attribute for use in distinguishing descriptions, that is, referring expressions produced with the purpose of identifying an object within a visual scene. However, most of these observations were based on visual displays containing only colors that were maximally different in hue and for which the language of experimentation possessed basic color terms. The experiments described in this paper investigate whether speakers’ preference for color is reduced if the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  40.  76
    Imposing Cognitive Constraints on Reference Production: The Interplay Between Speech and Gesture During Grounding.Ingrid Masson-Carro, Martijn Goudbeek & Emiel Krahmer - 2016 - Topics in Cognitive Science 8 (4):819-836.
    Past research has sought to elucidate how speakers and addressees establish common ground in conversation, yet few studies have focused on how visual cues such as co-speech gestures contribute to this process. Likewise, the effect of cognitive constraints on multimodal grounding remains to be established. This study addresses the relationship between the verbal and gestural modalities during grounding in referential communication. We report data from a collaborative task where repeated references were elicited, and a time constraint was imposed to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41.  86
    Visible Cohesion: A Comparison of Reference Tracking in Sign, Speech, and Co‐Speech Gesture.Pamela Perniss & Asli Özyürek - 2015 - Topics in Cognitive Science 7 (1):36-60.
    Establishing and maintaining reference is a crucial part of discourse. In spoken languages, differential linguistic devices mark referents occurring in different referential contexts, that is, introduction, maintenance, and re-introduction contexts. Speakers using gestures as well as users of sign languages have also been shown to mark referents differentially depending on the referential context. This article investigates the modality-specific contribution of the visual modality in marking referential context by providing a direct comparison between sign language and co-speech gesture with (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  42. Perceptual Co-Reference.Michael Rescorla - 2020 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 11 (3):569-589.
    The perceptual system estimates distal conditions based upon proximal sensory input. It typically exploits information from multiple cues across and within modalities: it estimates shape based upon visual and haptic cues; it estimates depth based upon convergence, binocular disparity, motion parallax, and other visual cues; and so on. Bayesian models illuminate the computations through which the perceptual system combines sensory cues. I review key aspects of these models. Based on my review, I argue that we should posit co-referring (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  43.  59
    Inscrutability and visual objects.Ben Phillips - 2017 - Synthese 194 (8):2949-2971.
    The thesis that the visual system represents objects has garnered empirical support from a variety of sources in recent decades. But what kinds of things qualify as “objects” in the relevant sense? Are they ordinary three-dimensional bodies? Are they the facing surfaces of three-dimensional bodies? I argue that there is no fact of the matter: what we have are equally acceptable ways of assigning extensions to the relevant visual states. The view I defend bears obvious similarities to Quine’s (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  19
    Color in Reference Production: The Role of Color Similarity and Color Codability.Jette Viethen, Thomas van Vessem, Martijn Goudbeek & Emiel Krahmer - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S6):1493-1514.
    It has often been observed that color is a highly preferred attribute for use in distinguishing descriptions, that is, referring expressions produced with the purpose of identifying an object within a visual scene. However, most of these observations were based on visual displays containing only colors that were maximally different in hue and for which the language of experimentation possessed basic color terms. The experiments described in this paper investigate whether speakers’ preference for color is reduced if the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  45.  25
    Rational Redundancy in Referring Expressions: Evidence from Event‐related Potentials.Elli N. Tourtouri, Francesca Delogu & Matthew W. Crocker - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (12):e13071.
    In referential communication, Grice's Maxim of Quantity is thought to imply that utterances conveying unnecessary information should incur comprehension difficulties. There is, however, considerable evidence that speakers frequently encode redundant information in their referring expressions, raising the question as to whether such overspecifications hinder listeners’ processing. Evidence from previous work is inconclusive, and mostly comes from offline studies. In this article, we present two event‐related potential (ERP) experiments, investigating the real‐time comprehension of referring expressions that contain redundant adjectives in complex (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  25
    The social basis of referential communication: Speakers construct physical reference based on listeners’ expected visual search.Julian Jara-Ettinger & Paula Rubio-Fernandez - 2022 - Psychological Review 129 (6):1394-1413.
  47.  22
    How social vs. visual perspective-taking determine the interpretation of linguistic reference by 8-11-year-olds with ASD and age-matched peers. [REVIEW]Kirsten Abbot-Smith, David M. Williams, Danielle Matthews, Lucy Pettifor & Nicola Vince - unknown
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  26
    Multi-modal referring expressions in human-human task descriptions and their implications for human-robot interaction.Stephanie Gross, Brigitte Krenn & Matthias Scheutz - 2016 - Interaction Studies 17 (2):180-210.
    Human instructors often refer to objects and actions involved in a task description using both linguistic and non-linguistic means of communication. Hence, for robots to engage in natural human-robot interactions, we need to better understand the various relevant aspects of human multi-modal task descriptions. We analyse reference resolution to objects in a data collection comprising two object manipulation tasks and find that 78.76% of all referring expressions to the objects relevant in Task 1 are verbally underspecified and 88.64% of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  18
    The perception of the vertical. IV. Adjustment to the vertical with normal and tilted visual frames of reference.George E. Passey - 1950 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 40 (6):738.
  50.  29
    Referential processing in monologue and dialogue with and without access to real world referents.S. C. Garrod - 2011 - In Edward Gibson & Neal J. Pearlmutter, The Processing and Acquisition of Reference. MIT Press. pp. 273--294.
    This chapter examines the role of the situation model in referential processing and how it can link what appear to be incompatible results from studies of monologue and dialogue as well as studies of reading and visual-world eye tracking. It shows that data from experiments on pronoun resolution in reading indicate a two-step model, in which candidate antecedents for an anaphor are first identified on the basis of gender matching and number matching, then evaluated with respect to the overall (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 971