Results for 'cognitive control'

979 found
Order:
  1. The cognitive control of emotion.K. N. Ochsner & J. J. Gross - 2005 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (5):242-249.
    The capacity to control emotion is important for human adaptation. Questions about the neural bases of emotion regulation have recently taken on new importance, as functional imaging studies in humans have permitted direct investigation of control strategies that draw upon higher cognitive processes difficult to study in nonhumans. Such studies have examined (1) controlling attention to, and (2) cognitively changing the meaning of, emotionally evocative stimuli. These two forms of emotion regulation depend upon interactions between prefrontal and (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   216 citations  
  2.  9
    Can Cognitive Control and Attentional Biases Explain More of the Variance in Depressive Symptoms Than Behavioral Processes? A Path Analysis Approach.Audrey Krings, Jessica Simon, Arnaud Carré & Sylvie Blairy - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13:809387.
    BackgroundThis study explored the proportion of variance in depressive symptoms explained by processes targeted by BA (activation, behavioral avoidance, anticipatory pleasure, and brooding), and processes targeted by cognitive control training (cognitive control, attentional biases, and brooding).MethodsFive hundred and twenty adults were recruited. They completed a spatial cueing task as a measure of attentional biases and a cognitive task as a measure of cognitive control and completed self-report measures of activation, behavioral avoidance, anticipatory pleasure, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3. Cognitive control in the self-regulation of physical activity and sedentary behavior.Jude Buckley, Jason D. Cohen, Arthur F. Kramer, Edward McAuley & Sean P. Mullen - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:104230.
    Cognitive control of physical activity and sedentary behavior is receiving increased attention in the neuroscientific and behavioral medicine literature as a means of better understanding and improving the self-regulation of physical activity. Enhancing individuals’ cognitive control capacities may provide a resilient means to increase physical activity and reduce sedentary behavior. First, this paper reviews emerging evidence of the antecedence of cognitive control abilities in successful self-regulation of physical activity, and in precipitating self-regulation failure that (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  4.  42
    Cognitive control, cognitive reserve, and memory in the aging bilingual brain.Angela Grant, Nancy A. Dennis & Ping Li - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:105591.
    In recent years bilingualism has been linked to both advantages in executive control and positive impacts on aging. Such positive cognitive effects of bilingualism have been attributed to the increased need for language control during bilingual processing and increased cognitive reserve, respectively. However, a mechanistic explanation of how bilingual experience contributes to cognitive reserve is still lacking. The current paper proposes a new focus on bilingual memory as an avenue to explore the relationship between executive (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  5.  32
    Cognitive control and cortisol response to stress in generalised anxiety disorder: a study of working memory capacity with negative and neutral distractors.Joelle LeMoult, Randi E. McCabe, Atayeh Hamedani & K. Lira Yoon - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 34 (4):800-806.
    We investigated the association between cognitive control and individual differences in cortisol response to stress in participants with generalised anxiety disorder and in never-disordered c...
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  25
    Cognitive Control as a 5-HT1A-Based Domain That Is Disrupted in Major Depressive Disorder.Scott A. Langenecker, Brian J. Mickey, Peter Eichhammer, Srijan Sen, Kathleen H. Elverman, Susan E. Kennedy, Mary M. Heitzeg, Saulo M. Ribeiro, Tiffany M. Love, David T. Hsu, Robert A. Koeppe, Stanley J. Watson, Huda Akil, David Goldman, Margit Burmeister & Jon-Kar Zubieta - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:441648.
    Heterogeneity within MDD has hampered identification of biological markers (e.g., intermediate phenotypes, IPs) that might increase risk for the disorder or reflect closer links to the genes underlying the disease process. The newer characterizations of dimensions of MDD within Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) domains may align well with the goal of defining IPs. We compare a sample of 25 individuals with MDD compared to 29 age and education matched controls in multimodal assessment. The multimodal RDoC assessment included the primary IP (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  17
    Cognitive Control in Suicide Ideators and Suicide Attempters.Silje Støle Brokke, Nils Inge Landrø & Vegard Øksendal Haaland - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    There is a need to understand more of the risk factors involved in the process from suicide ideation to suicide attempt. Cognitive control processes may be important factors in assessing vulnerability to suicide. A version of the Stroop procedure, Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System Color–Word Interference Test and Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function were used in this study to test attention control and cognitive shift, as well as to assess everyday executive function of 98 acute suicidal (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8. Cognitive Control: Easy to Identify But Hard to Define.J. Bruce Morton, Fredrick Ezekiel & Heather A. Wilk - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (2):212-216.
    Cognitive control is easy to identify in its effects, but difficult to grasp conceptually. This creates somewhat of a puzzle: Is cognitive control a bona fide process or an epiphenomenon that merely exists in the mind of the observer? The topiCS special edition on cognitive control presents a broad set of perspectives on this issue and helps to clarify central conceptual and empirical challenges confronting the field. Our commentary provides a summary of and critical (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  9.  49
    Enhancement of cognitive control by approach and avoidance motivational states.Adam C. Savine, Stefanie M. Beck, Bethany G. Edwards, Kimberly S. Chiew & Todd S. Braver - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (2):338-356.
    Affective variables have been shown to impact working memory and cognitive control. Theoretical arguments suggest that the functional impact of emotion on cognition might be mediated through shifting action dispositions related to changes in motivational orientation. The current study examined the effects of positive and negative affect on performance via direct manipulation of motivational state in tasks with high demands on cognitive control. Experiment 1 examined the effects of monetary reward on task-switching performance, while Experiment 2 (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  10. The Other Side of Cognitive Control: Can a Lack of Cognitive Control Benefit Language and Cognition?Evangelia G. Chrysikou, Jared M. Novick, John C. Trueswell & Sharon L. Thompson-Schill - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (2):253-256.
    Cognitive control refers to the regulation of mental activity to support flexible cognition across different domains. Cragg and Nation (2010) propose that the development of cognitive control in children parallels the development of language abilities, particularly inner speech. We suggest that children’s late development of cognitive control also mirrors their limited ability to revise misinterpretations of sentence meaning. Moreover, we argue that for certain tasks, a tradeoff between bottom-up (data-driven) and top-down (rule-based) thinking may (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  11.  94
    Cognitive Control: Componential and Yet Emergent.Ion Juvina - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (2):242-246.
    In this commentary, I will argue that the componential and emergent views of cognitive control as defined by Cooper (2010) do not necessarily oppose each other, and I will try to make a case for their interdependence. First, I will use the construct of cognitive inhibition—one of the main componential control functions mentioned in the target articles—to illustrate my line of reasoning. Then, I will comment on how some of the target articles, each from a different (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  12.  34
    Cognitive Control: Dynamic, Sustained, and Voluntary Influences.MaryBeth Knight - unknown
    The cost of incongruent stimuli is reduced when conflict is expected. This series of experiments tested whether this improved performance is due to repetition priming or to enhanced cognitive control. Using a paradigm in which Word and Number Stroop alternated every trial, Experiment 1 assessed dynamic trial-to-trial changes. Incongruent trials led to task-specific reduction of conflict (trial n ϩ 2) without cross-task modulation (trial n ϩ 1), but this was fully explained by repetition priming. In contrast, an increased (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  13.  45
    Cognitive control in romantic love: the roles of infatuation and attachment in interference and adaptive cognitive control.Sandra J. E. Langeslag & Henk van Steenbergen - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 34 (3):596-603.
    ABSTRACTBesides physiological, behavioural, and affective effects, romantic love also has cognitive effects. In this study, we tested whether individual differences in infatuation and/or attachment level predict impaired interference control even in the absence of a love booster procedure, and whether individual differences in attachment level predict reduced adaptive cognitive control as measured by conflict adaptation and post-error slowing. Eighty-three young adults who had recently fallen in love completed a Stroop-like task, which yielded reliable indices of interference (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  14.  49
    Cognitive control over learning: Creating, clustering, and generalizing task-set structure.Anne G. E. Collins & Michael J. Frank - 2013 - Psychological Review 120 (1):190-229.
  15. Cognitive Control: Componential or Emergent?Richard P. Cooper - 2010 - Topics in Cognitive Science 2 (4):598-613.
    The past 25 years have witnessed an increasing awareness of the importance of cognitive control in the regulation of complex behavior. It now sits alongside attention, memory, language, and thinking as a distinct domain within cognitive psychology. At the same time it permeates each of these sibling domains. This introduction reviews recent work on cognitive control in an attempt to provide a context for the fundamental question addressed within this topic: Is cognitive control (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  16.  18
    Cognitive control constrains memory attributions.Colleen M. Kelley & Larry L. Jacoby - 2019 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 42.
    Cognitive control constrains retrieval processing and so restricts what comes to mind as input to the attribution system. We review evidence that older adults, patients with Alzheimer's disease, and people with traumatic brain injury exert less cognitive control during retrieval, and so are susceptible to memory misattributions in the form of dramatic levels of false remembering.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  47
    Cognitive control acts locally.Wim Notebaert & Tom Verguts - 2008 - Cognition 106 (2):1071-1080.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  18. Cognitive Control and Flexibility in the Context of Stress and Depressive Symptoms: The Cognitive Control and Flexibility Questionnaire.Robert L. Gabrys, Nassim Tabri, Hymie Anisman & Kimberly Matheson - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
  19.  54
    The Computational and Neural Basis of Cognitive Control: Charted Territory and New Frontiers.Matthew M. Botvinick - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1249-1285.
    Cognitive control has long been one of the most active areas of computational modeling work in cognitive science. The focus on computational models as a medium for specifying and developing theory predates the PDP books, and cognitive control was not one of the areas on which they focused. However, the framework they provided has injected work on cognitive control with new energy and new ideas. On the occasion of the books' anniversary, we review (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  20.  9
    Cognitive control as a domain-general mechanism of school learning and development: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence.Gregoire Borst - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  21.  36
    Cognitive control of emotional distraction – valence-specific or general?Elisa Straub, Andrea Kiesel & David Dignath - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 34 (4):807-821.
    Emotional information captures attention due to privileged processing. Consequently, performance in cognitive tasks declines. Therefore, shielding current goals fro...
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  22.  22
    Cognitive Control and Bilingualism: The Bilingual Advantage Through the Lens of Dimensional Overlap.Melodie Bellegarda & Pedro Macizo - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Past research shows that the bilingual experience may enhance cognitive executive function. In this experiment, we evaluated cognitive control in bilinguals relative to monolinguals by using a dimensional overlap model to predict performance in a task composed of Stroop and Simon stimuli. A group of 24 Spanish monolinguals and 24 bilinguals with differing first languages and all having Spanish as a second language did a picture naming task and a task composed of Stroop and Simon stimuli, where (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  70
    Enhanced conflict-driven cognitive control by emotional arousal, not by valence.Qinghong Zeng, Senqing Qi, Miaoyun Li, Shuxia Yao, Cody Ding & Dong Yang - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (6):1083-1096.
    Emotion is widely agreed to have two dimensions, valence and arousal. Few studies have explored the effect of emotion on conflict adaptation by considering both of these, which could have dissociate influence. The present study aimed to fill the gap as to whether emotional valence and arousal would exert dissociable influence on conflict adaptation. In the experiments, we included positive, neutral, and negative conditions, with comparable arousal between positive and negative conditions. Both positive and negative conditions have higher arousal than (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  24.  29
    Cognitive control outside of conscious awareness.Adriano Linzarini, Olivier Houdé & Grégoire Borst - 2017 - Consciousness and Cognition 53:185-193.
  25. Cognitive control processes and hypnosis.Tobias Egner & Amir Raz - 2007 - In Graham A. Jamieson (ed.), Hypnosis and Conscious States: The Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press UK. pp. 29-50.
  26.  22
    The role of cognitive control mechanisms in selective attention towards emotional stimuli.Manuel Petrucci & Anna Pecchinenda - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (7):1480-1492.
    The role of cognitive control mechanisms in reducing interference from emotionally salient distractors was investigated. In two experiments, participants performed a flanker task in which target-distractor affective compatibility and cognitive load were manipulated. Differently from past studies, targets and distractors were presented at separate spatial locations and cognitive load was not domain-specific. In Experiment 1, words and faces, were used respectively as targets and distractors, whereas in Experiment 2, both targets and distractors were faces. Findings showed (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  27.  15
    Cognitive Control of Walking in Aging.Meltem Izzetoglu & Roee Holtzer - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  28.  19
    Expectancy to Eat Modulates Cognitive Control and Attention Toward Irrelevant Food and Non-food Images in Healthy Starving Individuals. A Behavioral Study.Sami Schiff, Giulia Testa, Maria Luisa Rusconi, Paolo Angeli & Daniela Mapelli - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    It is thought that just as hunger itself, the expectancy to eat impacts attention and cognitive control toward food stimuli, but this theory has not been extensively explored at a behavioral level. In order to study the effect of expectancy to eat on attentional and cognitive control mechanisms, 63 healthy fasting participants were presented with an affective priming spatial compatibility Simon task that included both food and object distracters. The participants were randomly assigned to two groups: (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29. Cognitive Control: Social Evolution and Emotional Regulation.Matt J. Rossano - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (2):238-241.
    This commentary argues that theories of cognitive control risk being incomplete unless they incorporate social/emotional factors. Social factors very likely played a critical role in the evolution of human cognitive control abilities, and emotional states are the primary regulatory mechanisms of cognitive control.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  48
    Prolonged Interruption of Cognitive Control of Conflict Processing Over Human Faces by Task-Irrelevant Emotion Expression.Jinyoung Kim, Min-Suk Kang, Yang Seok Cho & Sang-Hun Lee - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8:261802.
    As documented by Darwin 150 years ago, emotion expressed in human faces readily draws our attention and promotes sympathetic emotional reactions. How do such reactions to the expression of emotion affect our goal-directed actions? Despite the substantial advance made in the neural mechanisms of both cognitive control and emotional processing, it is not yet known well how these two systems interact. Here, we studied how emotion expressed in human faces influences cognitive control of conflict processing, spatial (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31. Defining an Ontology of Cognitive Control Requires Attention to Component Interactions.David Badre - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (2):217-221.
    Cognitive control is not only componential, but those components may interact in complicated ways in the service of cognitive control tasks. This complexity poses a challenge for developing an ontological description, because the mapping may not be direct between our task descriptions and true component differences reflected in indicators. To illustrate this point, I discuss two examples: (a) the relationship between adaptive gating and working memory and (b) the recent evidence for a control hierarchy. From (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  32.  51
    Cognitive Control of Episodic Memory in Schizophrenia: Differential Role of Dorsolateral and Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex.John D. Ragland, Charan Ranganath, Joshua Phillips, Megan A. Boudewyn, Ann M. Kring, Tyler A. Lesh, Debra L. Long, Steven J. Luck, Tara A. Niendam, Marjorie Solomon, Tamara Y. Swaab & Cameron S. Carter - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  33.  19
    Cognitive control and the non-conscious regulation of health behavior.Amanda L. Rebar, Andrea M. Loftus & Martin S. Hagger - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  34.  31
    Cognitive Control and Emotional Intelligence: Effect of the Emotional Content of the Task. Brief Reports.Purificación Checa & Pablo Fernández-Berrocal - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  23
    Cognitive Control and Ruminative Responses to Stress: Understanding the Different Facets of Cognitive Control.Bita Zareian, Jessica Wilson & Joelle LeMoult - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12:660062.
    Rumination has been linked to the onset and course of depression. Theoretical models and empirical evidence suggest that deficits controlling negative material in working memory underlie rumination. However, we do not know which component of cognitive control (inhibition, shifting, or updating) contributes most to rumination, and whether different components predict the more maladaptive (brooding) versus the more adaptive (reflection) forms of rumination. We aimed to advance theory and research by examining the contribution of different facets of cognitive (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  36.  6
    Inhibition in the emotional Hayling task: can hypnotic suggestion enhance cognitive control on a prepotent negative word?Jeremy Brunel, Sandrine Delord & Stéphanie Mathey - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    Recent studies suggest that instrumental hypnosis is a useful experimental tool to investigate emotional and language processing effects. However, the capacity of hypnotic suggestions to intervene during the response inhibition of emotional words remains elusive. This study investigated whether hypnotic suggestion can improve the inhibition of prepotent negative word responses in an emotional Hayling sentence completion task. High-suggestible participants performed a computerised emotional Hayling task. They were first asked to select the appropriate words ending highly predictable sentences among two propositions (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37. Considering the role of cognitive control in expert performance.John Toner, Barbara Gail Montero & Aidan Moran - 2015 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 14 (4):1127-1144.
    Dreyfus and Dreyfus’ influential phenomenological analysis of skill acquisition proposes that expert performance is guided by non-cognitive responses which are fast, effortless and apparently intuitive in nature. Although this model has been criticised for over-emphasising the role that intuition plays in facilitating skilled performance, it does recognise that on occasions a form of ‘detached deliberative rationality’ may be used by experts to improve their performance. However, Dreyfus and Dreyfus see no role for calculative problem solving or deliberation when performance (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  38.  45
    Studying the role of cognitive control in reasoning: evidence for the congruency sequence effect in the ratio-bias task.Balazs Aczel & Bence Palfi - 2017 - Thinking and Reasoning 23 (1):81-97.
    In this study, we investigated whether control of the conflict between incongruent heuristic and analytical answer options in a reasoning task is modulated by the presence of conflict on previous trials. In two experiments, we found that the incongruency of the previous trial has a significant effect on the control exhibited on the current trial. Our data also showed that this adaptation effect is modulated by the incongruency of the previous series of trials. These results demonstrate the same (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  39.  37
    Regulating cognitive control through approach-avoidance motor actions.Severine Koch, Rob W. Holland & Ad van Knippenberg - 2008 - Cognition 109 (1):133-142.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  40.  21
    Cognitive Control, Cognitive Biases and Emotion Regulation in Depression: A New Proposal for an Integrative Interplay Model.Dolores Villalobos, Javier Pacios & Carmelo Vázquez - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Research traditions on cognition and depression focus on relatively unconnected aspects of cognitive functioning. On one hand, the neuropsychological perspective has concentrated on cognitive control difficulties as a prominent feature of this condition. On the other hand, the clinical psychology perspective has focused on cognitive biases and repetitive negative patterns of thinking for emotional information. A review of the literature from both fields reveals that difficulties are more evident for mood-congruent materials, suggesting that cognitive (...) difficulties interact with cognitive biases to hinder cognitive switching, working memory updating, and inhibition of irrelevant information. Connecting research from these two traditions, we propose a novel integrative cognitive model of depression in which the interplay between mood-congruent cognitive control difficulties, cognitive biases, and rumination may ultimately lead to ineffective emotion-regulation strategies to downregulate negative mood and upregulate positive mood. (shrink)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  41. Conscious cognitive effort in cognitive control.Joshua Shepherd - forthcoming - WIREs Cognitive Science.
    Cognitive effort is thought to be familiar in everyday life, ubiquitous across multiple variations of task and circumstance, and integral to cost/benefit computations that are themselves central to the proper functioning of cognitive control. In particular, cognitive effort is thought to be closely related to the assessment of cognitive control’s costs. I argue here that the construct of cognitive effort, as it is deployed in cognitive psychology and neuroscience, is problematically unclear. The (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  29
    Phasic affective signals by themselves do not regulate cognitive control.Miklos Bognar, Mate Gyurkovics, Henk van Steenbergen & Balazs Aczel - 2023 - Cognition and Emotion 37 (4):650-665.
    Cognitive control is a set of mechanisms that help us process conflicting stimuli and maintain goal-relevant behaviour. According to the Affective Signalling Hypothesis, conflicting stimuli are aversive and thus elicit (negative) affect, moreover – to avoid aversive signals – affective and cognitive systems work together by increasing control and thus, drive conflict adaptation. Several studies have found that affective stimuli can indeed modulate conflict adaptation, however, there is currently no evidence that phasic affective states not triggered (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  43.  20
    A Robotic Cognitive Control Framework for Collaborative Task Execution and Learning.Riccardo Caccavale & Alberto Finzi - 2022 - Topics in Cognitive Science 14 (2):327-343.
    Topics in Cognitive Science, Volume 14, Issue 2, Page 327-343, April 2022.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  79
    Cognitive control in altruism and self-control: A social cognitive neuroscience perspective.Jeremy R. Gray & Todd S. Braver - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (2):260-260.
    The primrose path and prisoner's dilemma paradigms may require cognitive (executive) control: The active maintenance of context representations in lateral prefrontal cortex to provide top-down support for specific behaviors in the face of short delays or stronger response tendencies. This perspective suggests further tests of whether altruism is a type of self-control, including brain imaging, induced affect, and dual-task studies.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  16
    Cognitive control and automatic interference in mind and brain: A unified model of saccadic inhibition and countermanding.Aline Bompas, Anne Eileen Campbell & Petroc Sumner - 2020 - Psychological Review 127 (4):524-561.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  18
    Quantifying Contextual Information For Cognitive Control.Francisco Barceló & Patrick S. Cooper - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Cognition is context-sensitive, as the same sensory information is processed differently depending on its context (e.g., on its probabilistic association with goal-directed actions and their outcomes). Despite this, the concept of context in studies of higher-order cognitive processes, like cognitive control, is often simplified to nominal stimulus categories (like a target vs. distractor). Here we propose that quantifying contextual information to model cognitive demands is (1) fruitful as it can provide novel insight into the nature of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  47.  49
    Memory and cognitive control in an integrated theory of language processing.L. Robert Slevc & Jared M. Novick - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (4):373-374.
    Pickering & Garrod's (P&G's) integrated model of production and comprehension includes no explicit role for nonlinguistic cognitive processes. Yet, how domain-general cognitive functions contribute to language processing has become clearer with well-specified theories and supporting data. We therefore believe that their account can benefit by incorporating functions like working memory and cognitive control into a unified model of language processing.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  48.  31
    Cognitive control in action: Tracking the dynamics of rule switching in 5- to 8-year-olds and adults.Christopher D. Erb, Jeff Moher, Joo-Hyun Song & David M. Sobel - 2017 - Cognition 164 (C):163-173.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  49. Language and the Development of Cognitive Control.Lucy Cragg & Kate Nation - 2010 - Topics in Cognitive Science 2 (4):631-642.
    We review the relationships between language, inner speech, and cognitive control in children and young adults, focusing on the domain of cognitive flexibility. We address the role that inner speech plays in flexibly shifting between tasks, addressing whether it is used to represent task rules, provide a reminder of task order, or aid in task retrieval. We also consider whether the development of inner speech in childhood serves to drive the development of cognitive flexibility. We conclude (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  50.  30
    Effects of Cognitive Control Exertion and Motor Coordination on Task Self-Efficacy and Muscular Endurance Performance in Children.Jeffrey D. Graham, Yao-Chuen Li, Steven R. Bray & John Cairney - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12:347028.
    Emerging research shows a strong connection between brain areas governing cognition and motor behavior. Yet, research investigating the negative aftereffects of cognitive control exertion on task performance has not considered the potential role of areas governing motor behavior. The present study investigated the effects of high cognitive control exertion on task self-efficacy and exercise performance in children. A secondary purpose was to investigate whether motor coordination influences the change in exercise performance differently following low versus high (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 979