Results for ' mood induction'

964 found
Order:
  1.  20
    Mood Induction Differently Affects Early Neural Correlates of Evaluative Word Processing in L1 and L2.Johanna Kissler & Katarzyna Bromberek-Dyzman - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    We investigate how mood inductions impact the neural processing of emotional adjectives in one’s first language and a formally acquired second language. Twenty-three student participants took part in an EEG experiment with two separate sessions. Happy or sad mood inductions were followed by series of individually presented positive, negative, or neutral adjectives in L1 or L2 and evaluative decisions had to be performed. Visual event-related potentials elicited during word processing were analyzed during N1, Early Posterior Negativities, N400, and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  2.  33
    Mood induction and the priming of semantic memory in a lexical decision task: Asymmetric effects of elation and depression.Bradford H. Challis & Richard V. Krane - 1988 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26 (4):309-312.
  3.  25
    Emotional Differences in Young and Older Adults: Films as Mood Induction Procedure.Luz Fernández-Aguilar, Jorge Ricarte, Laura Ros & Jose M. Latorre - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:372611.
    Film clips are proven to be one of the most efficient techniques in emotional induction. However, there is scant literature on the effect of this procedure in older adults and, specifically, the effect of using different positive stimuli. Thus, the aim of the present study was to examine emotional differences between young and older adults and to know how a set of film clips works as mood induction procedure in older adults, especially, when trying to elicit attachment-related (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  42
    Non-response to sad mood induction: implications for emotion research.Jonathan Rottenberg, Maria Kovacs & Ilya Yaroslavsky - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (3):431-436.
    Experimental induction of sad mood states is a mainstay of laboratory research on affect and cognition, mood regulation, and mood disorders. Typically, the success of such mood manipulations is reported as a statistically significant pre- to post-induction change in the self-rated intensity of the target affect. The present commentary was motivated by an unexpected finding in one of our studies concerning the response rate to a well-validated sad mood induction. Using the customary (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  5.  22
    The effect of mood induction in a risky decision-making task.Patricia J. Deldin & Irwin P. Levin - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (1):4-6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  43
    Web-based mood induction.Anja S. Göritz & Klaus Moser - 2006 - Cognition and Emotion 20 (6):887-896.
  7.  35
    Mood induction and instructions to sustain moods: A test of the subject compliance hypothesis of mood congruent memory.W. Gerrod Parrott - 1991 - Cognition and Emotion 5 (1):41-52.
  8.  45
    Validation of a music mood induction procedure: Some preliminary findings.Pamela Kenealy - 1988 - Cognition and Emotion 2 (1):41-48.
  9.  36
    The effect of depressed mood on the interpretation of ambiguity, with and without negative mood induction.M. A. Suzie Bisson & Christopher R. Sears - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (3):614-645.
    Is there an effect of depressed mood on the interpretation of ambiguity? Are depressed individuals biased to interpret ambiguous information in a negative manner? We used a cross-modal semantic priming task to look for evidence of a negative interpretative bias. Participants listened to ambiguous prime sentences (e.g., Joan was stunned by her final exam mark) and made lexical decisions to target words presented immediately after the sentence offset or after a delay of 1000 ms or 2000 ms. For the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  10.  34
    A convenient self-referencing mood induction procedure.Pennie S. Seibert & Henry C. Ellis - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (2):121-124.
  11.  20
    Self-report measures of defeat and entrapment during a brief depressive mood induction.Richard C. Goldstein & Paul Willner - 2002 - Cognition and Emotion 16 (5):629-642.
  12.  13
    The effect of depressed mood on the interpretation of ambiguity, with and without negative mood induction.Ma Suzie Bisson & Christopher R. Sears - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (3):614-645.
  13.  49
    Negative cognitive response to a sad mood induction: Associations with polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) gene.Christopher G. Beevers, Walter D. Scott, Chinatsu McGeary & John E. McGeary - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (4):726-738.
  14.  38
    Playing with fire: effects of negative mood induction and working memory on vocabulary acquisition.Zachary F. Miller, Jessica K. Fox, Jason S. Moser & Aline Godfroid - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (5):1105-1113.
    ABSTRACTWe investigated the impact of emotions on learning vocabulary in an unfamiliar language to better understand affective influences in foreign language acquisition. Seventy native English speakers learned new vocabulary in either a negative or a neutral emotional state. Participants also completed two sets of working memory tasks to examine the potential mediating role of working memory. Results revealed that participants exposed to negative stimuli exhibited difficulty in retrieving and correctly pairing English words with Indonesian words, as reflected in a lower (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  15.  39
    Mental state decoding in past major depression: Effect of sad versus happy mood induction.Kate L. Harkness, Jill A. Jacobson, David Duong & Mark A. Sabbagh - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (3):497-513.
  16.  88
    Sex Hormones Are Associated With Rumination and Interact With Emotion Regulation Strategy Choice to Predict Negative Affect in Women Following a Sad Mood Induction.Bronwyn M. Graham, Thomas F. Denson, Justine Barnett, Clare Calderwood & Jessica R. Grisham - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  17.  13
    Why might negative mood help or hinder inhibitory performance? An exploration of thinking styles using a Navon induction.Martyn Sean Gabel & Tara McAuley - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (4):705-712.
    Theories of affective influences on cognition posit that negative mood may increase cognitive load, causing a decrement in task performance (Seibert & Ellis, [1991]. Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states, and cognitive task performance. Memory & Cognition, 19(5), 507–513), or cause a shift to more analytic thinking, which benefits tasks requiring attention to detail (Schwarz & Clore, [1983]. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513–523). (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  20
    Is your mood more contagious if you are likeable? The role of liking in the social induction of affect.Klara Królewiak & Monika Wróbel - 2015 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 46 (3):413-420.
    In the present study, we explored the role of liking in the social induction of affect. Dispositional likeability was manipulated by written reports describing a sender as a likeable or dislikeable character. Afterwards participants watched short videos presenting the sender displaying happy or sad emotional expressions. We expected that exposure to the likeable sender would lead to reactions concordant with his emotional expression, whereas exposure to the dislikeable sender would result in discordant reactions. The results indicated that dispositional likeability (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  37
    Experimental mood manipulation does not induce change in preference for natural landscapes.Bernadette Klopp & Linda Mealey - 1998 - Human Nature 9 (4):391-399.
    According to evolutionary theory, emotions are psychological mechanisms that have evolved to enhance fitness in specific situations by motivating appropriate (adaptive) behavior. Taking this perspective, a previous study examined the relationship between mood and preference for natural environments. It reported that participants’ anxiety level was associated with a preference for landscapes offering what Appleton called "refuge," while participants’ anger and cheerfulness were both associated with a preference for landscapes offering what Appleton called "prospect." We attempted to replicate these results (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  50
    Social media’s influence on momentary emotion based on people’s initial mood: an experimental design.Alison B. Tuck, Kelley A. Long & Renee J. Thompson - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    Can you think of a meme that made you laugh or a political post that made you angry? These examples illustrate how social media use (SMU) impacts how people feel. Similarly, how people feel when they initiate SMU may impact the emotional effects of SMU. Someone feeling happy may feel more positively during SMU, whereas someone feeling sad may feel more negatively. Using an experimental design, we examined whether following SMU, those in a happy mood would experience increases in (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  21.  42
    Embodied mood regulation: the impact of body posture on mood recovery, negative thoughts, and mood-congruent recall.Lotte Veenstra, Iris K. Schneider & Sander L. Koole - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (7):1361-1376.
    ABSTRACTPrevious work has shown that a stooped posture may activate negative mood. Extending this work, the present experiments examine how stooped body posture influences recovery from pre-existing negative mood. In Experiment 1, participants were randomly assigned to receive either a negative or neutral mood induction, after which participants were instructed to take either a stooped, straight, or control posture while writing down their thoughts. Stooped posture led to less mood recovery in the negative mood (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  22.  33
    Mood state, task demand, and effort-related cardiovascular response.Guido H. E. Gendolla & Jan Krüsken - 2002 - Cognition and Emotion 16 (5):577-603.
    Drawing on the mood-behaviour model (Gendolla, 2000), two studies investigated informational effects of mood on effort-related cardiovascular response. Experiment 1 manipulated mood state (positive, negative) and task difficulty (easy, difficult, extremely difficult). Effects on cardiovascular reactivity were as expected: On the easy level, reactivity was weak in a positive mood, but strong in a negative mood; on the difficult level, reactivity was strong in a positive mood, but weak in a negative mood; on (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  23.  1
    Browse or broadcast? The influence of active and passive social media use on mood.Sophie H. Li, Brittany Corkish & Aliza Werner-Seidler - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    Associations between screen time and mental health may be driven by increased use in young people with heightened symptoms as a means of modifying negative mood. However, the direct effect of technology use on mood remains unclear. This study aimed to investigate the effect of active and passive social media use on an induced sad or neutral mood by randomising young people (16–24 years; N = 116) to a sad or neutral mood induction task and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  44
    The influence of mood on the intensity of emotional responses: Disentangling feeling and knowing.Roland Neumann, Beate Seibt & Fritz Strack - 2001 - Cognition and Emotion 15 (6):725-747.
    The results of three experiments suggest that pre-existing mood increases the intensity of affectively congruent emotions while dampening the intensity of incongruent emotions independent of attributional knowledge. This result was obtained using a new method for inducing mood states unobtrusively and with minimal or no cognitive concomitants. The results of Experiment 1 revealed that for participants who were exposed to positive feedback a pre-existing positive mood led to stronger feelings of pride in comparison to negative mood. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  25.  17
    How Drawing to Distract Improves Mood in Children.Jennifer E. Drake - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Previous research has shown that drawing improves short-term mood in children when used to distract from rather than express negative thoughts and feelings. The current study sought to examine how drawing might elevate mood in children ages 6–12 by examining the role played by absorption, enjoyment, and perceived competence as well as entering an imaginary world; and whether children spontaneously use drawing to distract from a sad mood. Across three studies, children were asked to think of a (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  16
    Differences in Mood, Optimism, and Risk-Taking Behavior Between American and Chinese College Students.Jiao Wang, Ruifeng Cui, Stephanie Stolarz-Fantino, Edmund Fantino & Xiaoming Liu - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Mood and optimism have been demonstrated to influence risk-taking decisions; however, the literature on mood, optimism, and decision-making is mixed and conducted primarily with western samples. This study sought to address this gap in the literature by examining the impact of mood and dispositional optimism on risk-taking and whether these associations differed between undergraduate students from the United States and the People’s Republic of China. Both samples completed a dispositional optimism questionnaire and an autobiographical mood (...) task. They were then tasked with choosing to complete the Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices reasoning task on easy, medium, or hard difficulty for hypothetical money. Selecting harder difficulties was interpreted as more risk-taking due to a higher chance of failure. More positive mood and higher dispositional optimism were associated with decreased risk-taking, i.e., selecting easier puzzle difficulties, in the American sample but increased risk-taking decisions, i.e., selecting harder difficulties, in the Chinese sample. These findings suggest that the effect of mood and optimism on decision-making may differ by nationality and/or culture. (shrink)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  29
    I feel good, therefore I am real: Testing the causal influence of mood on state authenticity.Alison P. Lenton, Letitia Slabu, Constantine Sedikides & Katherine Power - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (7):1202-1224.
    Although the literature has focused on individual differences in authenticity, recent findings suggest that authenticity is sensitive to context; that is, it is also a state. We extended this perspective by examining whether incidental affect influences authenticity. In three experiments, participants felt more authentic when in a relatively positive than negative mood. The causal role of affect in authenticity was consistent across a diverse set of mood inductions, including explicit (Experiments 1 and 3) and implicit (Experiment 2) methods. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  28.  7
    The Influence of Affective State on Subjective-Report Measurements: Evidence From Experimental Manipulations of Mood.Kine Askim & Stein Knardahl - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    A substantial portion of the knowledge base of psychology is based on subjective reports with a risk of information bias. The objective of the present study was to elucidate one contextual source of variance and potential bias in subjective reports: the influence of affective state at the time of responding to questionnaires. Employees were subjected to mood-induction procedures in the laboratory. Neutral, positive, and negative moods were induced by combinations of pictures from the international affective picture set and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  29.  21
    Effects of Sleep Fragmentation and Induced Mood on Pain Tolerance and Pain Sensitivity in Young Healthy Adults.Ragna Rosseland, Ståle Pallesen, Inger Hilde Nordhus, Dagfinn Matre & Tone Blågestad - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:356779.
    _Background:_ Experimental research exploring the sleep/pain-relationship has typically focused on total or partial sleep deprivation, hereby failing to reproduce the mere fragmented sleep pattern typically observed in patients with chronic pain. Further, little research is done on how affect moderates the sleep–pain relationship after sleep fragmentation. The present study sought to clarify the relationship between pain, sleep and positive and negative affect. _Methods:_ In an experimental counterbalanced crossover design, 35 healthy young adults were subjected to several pain measures after one (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  30.  12
    Unlocking the past: efficacy of guided self-compassion and benefit-focused online interventions for managing negative personal memories.Rosaria Maria Zangri, Ivan Blanco, Teodoro Pascual & Carmelo Vázquez - 2024 - Cognition and Emotion 38 (7):971-985.
    Positive reappraisal strategies have been found to reduce negative affect following the recall of negative personal events. This study examined the restorative effect of two mood-repair instructions (self-compassion vs benefit-focused reappraisal) and a control condition with no instructions following a negative Mood Induction Procedure by using the guided recall of a negative autobiographical event. A total of 112 university students participated in the online study (81% women, Mage: 21.0 years). Immediately following the negative memory recall, participants were (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  43
    Attachment styles and secure base priming in relation to emotional reactivity after frustration induction.Annemiek Karreman, Ad J. J. M. Vingerhoets & Marrie H. J. Bekker - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (3):428-441.
    ABSTRACTIn two experimental studies, we explored the role of attachment in predicting emotional reactivity after frustration induction. In the first study, using a cognitive frustration task, we examined in a college sample how attachment styles related to the experience and expression of emotions after frustration induction. In the second study, we investigated in college students the effect of conscious priming of the secure base schema on mood disturbance after the performance of a cognitive frustration task. Results showed (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  56
    Distinctive effects of fear and sadness induction on anger and aggressive behavior.Jun Zhan, Jun Ren, Jin Fan & Jing Luo - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:134592.
    A recent study has reported that the successful implementation of cognitive regulation of emotion depends on higher-level cognitive functions, such as top-down control, which may be impaired in stressful situations. This calls for “cognition free” self-regulatory strategies that do not require top-down control. In contrast to the cognitive regulation of emotion that emphasizes the role of cognition, traditional Chinese philosophy and medicine views the relationship among different types of emotions as promoting or counteracting each other without the involvement of cognition, (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  33.  50
    Do negative mood states impact moral reasoning?Brian Barger & W. Pitt Derryberry - 2013 - Journal of Moral Education 42 (4):443-459.
    This paper presents three studies exploring the relationship between emotional responses to classic cognitive developmental moral dilemmas and moral reasoning indices as measured by the Defining Issues Test (DIT). Each study indicated that certain moral dilemmas elicit varying levels of anger and sadness as compared to a neutral baseline. In each study, decreased moral reasoning was observed in those instances where reports in both sadness and anger were high following a dilemma. This did not occur, however, in those instances where (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  34. The Logic of Causation: Definition, Induction and Deduction of Deterministic Causality.Avi Sion - 1999 - Geneva, Switzerland: CreateSpace & Kindle; Lulu..
    The Logic of Causation: Definition, Induction and Deduction of Deterministic Causality is a treatise of formal logic and of aetiology. It is an original and wide-ranging investigation of the definition of causation (deterministic causality) in all its forms, and of the deduction and induction of such forms. The work was carried out in three phases over a dozen years (1998-2010), each phase introducing more sophisticated methods than the previous to solve outstanding problems. This study was intended as part (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  13
    Brain Responses to a Self-Compassion Induction in Trauma Survivors With and Without Post-traumatic Stress Disorder.Jennifer L. Creaser, Joanne Storr & Anke Karl - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Self-compassion is a mechanism of symptom improvement in post-traumatic stress disorder, however, the underlying neurobiological processes are not well understood. High levels of self-compassion are associated with reduced activation of the threat response system. Physiological threat responses to trauma reminders and increased arousal are key symptoms which are maintained by negative appraisals of the self and self-blame. Moreover, PTSD has been consistently associated with functional changes implicated in the brain’s saliency and the default mode networks. In this paper, we explore (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  2
    Seeing fast and slow: the influence of music-induced affective states and individual sensory sensitivity on visual processing speed.Gaia Lapomarda, Michele Deodato & David Melcher - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    There is a speed-accuracy trade-off in perception. The ability to quickly extract sensory information is critical for survival, while extended processing can improve our accuracy. It has been suggested that emotions can change our style of processing, but their influence on processing speed is not yet clear. In three experiments, combining online and laboratory studies with different emotion induction procedures, we investigated the influence of both affective states, manipulated with music, and individual traits in sensory-processing sensitivity on the ability (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  35
    Internally and externally generated emotions in people with acquired brain injury: preservation of emotional experience after right hemisphere lesions.Christian E. Salas Riquelme, Darinka Radovic, Osvaldo Castro & Oliver H. Turnbull - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:121484.
    The study of emotional changes after brain injury has contributed enormously to the understanding of the neural basis of emotion. However, little attention has been placed on the methods used to elicit emotional responses in people with brain damage. Of particular interest are subjects with right hemisphere [RH] cortical lesions, who have been described as presenting impairment in emotional processing. In this article, an internal and external mood induction procedure [MIP] was used to trigger positive and negative emotions, (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  18
    Boredom and Media Multitasking.Allison C. Drody, Brandon C. W. Ralph, James Danckert & Daniel Smilek - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Media multitasking entails simultaneously engaging in multiple tasks when at least one of the tasks involves media. Across two studies, we investigated one potential trigger of media multitasking, state boredom, and its relation to media multitasking. To this end, we manipulated participants’ levels of state boredom using video mood inductions prior to administering an attention-demanding 2-back task during which participants could media multitask by playing a task-irrelevant video. We also examined whether trait boredom proneness was associated media multitasking. We (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  43
    Autobiographical memory characteristics in depression vulnerability: Formerly depressed individuals recall less vivid positive memories.Aliza Werner-Seidler & Michelle L. Moulds - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (6):1087-1103.
    The differential activation hypothesis (DAH; Teasdale, 1988) proposes that individuals who are vulnerable to depression can be distinguished from non-vulnerable individuals by the degree to which negative thoughts and maladaptive cognitive processes are activated during sad mood. While retrieval of negative autobiographical memories is noted as one such process, the model does not articulate a role for deficits in recalling positive memories. Two studies were conducted to compare the autobiographical memory characteristics of never-depressed and formerly depressed individuals following a (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  40.  51
    The effects of emotion regulation strategies on positive and negative affect in early adolescents.Laura Wante, Marie-Lotte Van Beveren, Lotte Theuwis & Caroline Braet - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (5):988-1002.
    ABSTRACTRecent research suggests that impaired emotion regulation may play an important role in the development of youth psychopathology. However, little research has explored the effects of ER strategies on affect in early adolescents. In Study 1, we examined if early adolescents are able to use distraction and whether the effects of this strategy are similar to talking to one’s mother. In Study 2, we compared the effects of distraction, cognitive reappraisal, acceptance, and rumination. In both studies, participants received instructions on (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  41.  26
    Anxiety and disgust: Evidence for a unidirectional relationship.Sarah Marzillier & Graham Davey - 2005 - Cognition and Emotion 19 (5):729-750.
    This paper reports the results of three studies using mood induction procedures (MIPs) designed to investigate the relationship between anxiety and disgust. Study 1 used guided imagery vignettes (i.e., asking participants to imagine themselves in a series of described situations) and music (Mayer, Allen, & Beauregard, 1995). Study 2 used video clips (Gross & Levenson, 1995). Study 3 used autobiographical recall and music (Blagden & Craske, 1996). In order to be as sure as possible that target moods were (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  42.  40
    Early-emerging cognitive vulnerability to depression and the serotonin transporter promoter region polymorphism.E. P. Hayden, L. R. Dougherty, B. Maloney, T. M. Olino, H. Sheikh, C. E. Durbin, J. I. Nurnberger Jr, D. K. Lahiri & D. N. Klein - 2008 - J Affect Disord 107:227-30.
    BACKGROUND: Serotonin transporter promoter genotype appears to increase risk for depression in the context of stressful life events. However, the effects of this genotype on measures of stress sensitivity are poorly understood. Therefore, this study examined whether 5-HTTLPR genotype was associated with negative information processing biases in early childhood. METHOD: Thirty-nine unselected seven-year-old children completed a negative mood induction procedure and a Self-Referent Encoding Task designed to measure positive and negative schematic processing. Children were also genotyped for the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  43.  24
    Experimental clues in favor of a generalized norm of reciprocity.Boris Vallée, Nicolas Guéguen, Mickaël Dupré & Sebastien Meineri - 2017 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 48 (3):375-379.
    The norm of reciprocity has only been considered and experimentally demonstrated between two individuals. However, comments from several authors invite the consideration of an expanded form of this norm. 120 passersby, randomly assigned to 3 conditions, were asked to watch a confederate’s belongings. Depending on the condition, they had either previously been given a gift or not. In addition, the gift was offered by either the confederate making the target request or by a second confederate, not initially involved. First, results (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  14
    Perceived vs. Actual Emotion Reactivity and Regulation in Individuals With and Without a History of NSSI.Jessica Mettler, Melissa Stern, Stephen P. Lewis & Nancy L. Heath - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Non-suicidal self-injury has consistently been associated with self-reported difficulties in emotion reactivity and the regulation of negative emotions; however, less is known about the accuracy of these self-reports or the reactivity and regulation of positive emotions. The present study sought to investigate differences between women with and without a history of NSSI on: self-reported general tendencies of negative and positive emotion reactivity, self-reported general tendencies of negative and positive emotion regulation, and emotion regulation reported in response to a positive and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  2
    Emotional inertia is independently associated with cognitive emotion regulation strategies and sleep quality.Emma Caitlin Sullivan, Cade McCall, Annette Brose, Lisa-Marie Henderson & Scott Ashley Cairney - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    Emotional inertia (i.e. the tendency for emotions to persist over time) is robustly associated with lower wellbeing. Yet, we know little about the mechanisms underlying this relationship. Good quality sleep and frequent use of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation (CER) strategies reduce the persistence of negative affect (NA) over time. However, whether sleep and adaptive CER strategy use work in concert to reduce NA inertia is unclear. In the current study, participants (N = 245) watched a series of film clips and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  39
    Utilizing Neutral Affective States in Research: Theory, Assessment, and Recommendations.Karen Gasper - 2018 - Emotion Review 10 (3):255-266.
    Even though researchers regularly use neutral affect induction procedures as a control condition in their work, there is little consensus on what is neutral affect. This article reviews five approaches that researchers have used to operationalize neutral AIPs: to produce a minimal affective state, in-the-middle state, deactivated state, typical state, or indifferent state. For each view, the article delineates the theoretical basis for the neutral AIP, how to assess it, and provides recommendations for when and how to use it. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  47. Schemata: The concept of schema in the history of logic.John Corcoran - 2006 - Bulletin of Symbolic Logic 12 (2):219-240.
    The syllogistic figures and moods can be taken to be argument schemata as can the rules of the Stoic propositional logic. Sentence schemata have been used in axiomatizations of logic only since the landmark 1927 von Neumann paper [31]. Modern philosophers know the role of schemata in explications of the semantic conception of truth through Tarski’s 1933 Convention T [42]. Mathematical logicians recognize the role of schemata in first-order number theory where Peano’s second-order Induction Axiom is approximated by Herbrand’s (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  48.  55
    Manipulating affective state influences conditioned appetitive responses.Inna Arnaudova, Angelos-Miltiadis Krypotos, Marieke Effting, Merel Kindt & Tom Beckers - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (5):1062-1081.
    ABSTRACTAffective states influence how individuals process information and behave. Some theories predict emotional congruency effects. Emotional congruency should theoretically obstruct the learning of reward associations and their ability to guide behaviour under negative mood. Two studies tested the effects of the induction of a negative affective state on appetitive Pavlovian learning, in which neutral stimuli were associated with chocolate or alcohol rewards. In both experiments, participants showed enhanced approach tendencies towards predictors of reward after a negative relative to (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  49
    ‘Woe Betides Anybody Who Tries to Turn me Down.’ A Qualitative Analysis of Neuropsychiatric Symptoms Following Subthalamic Deep Brain Stimulation for Parkinson’s Disease.Philip E. Mosley, Katherine Robinson, Terry Coyne, Peter Silburn, Michael Breakspear & Adrian Carter - 2019 - Neuroethics 14 (1):47-63.
    Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus for the treatment of Parkinson’s disease can lead to the development of neuropsychiatric symptoms. These can include harmful changes in mood and behaviour that alienate family members and raise ethical questions about personal responsibility for actions committed under stimulation-dependent mental states. Qualitative interviews were conducted with twenty participants following subthalamic DBS at a movement disorders centre, in order to explore the meaning and significance of stimulation-related neuropsychiatric symptoms amongst a purposive sample of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  50.  45
    BRIEF REPORT Gratitude and prosocial behaviour: An experimental test of gratitude.Jo-Ann Tsang - 2006 - Cognition and Emotion 20 (1):138-148.
    McCullough, Kilpatrick, Emmons, and Larson (2001) posited that gratitude prompts individuals to behave prosocially. However, research supporting the prosocial effect of gratitude has relied on scenario and self-report methodology. To address limitations of previous research, this experiment utilised a laboratory induction of gratitude, a method that is potentially more covert than scenarios and that elicits actual grateful emotion. Prosocial responses to gratitude—operationalised as the distribution of resources to another—were paired with a self-report measure of gratitude to test the prosocial (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
1 — 50 / 964