Order:
Disambiguations
R. Hans Phaf [10]R. H. Phaf [4]R. Phaf [3]
  1.  50
    Approach, avoidance, and affect: a meta-analysis of approach-avoidance tendencies in manual reaction time tasks.R. Hans Phaf, Sören E. Mohr, Mark Rotteveel & Jelte M. Wicherts - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
  2. A constructivist and connectionist view on conscious and nonconscious processes.R. Hans Phaf & Gezinus Wolters - 1997 - Philosophical Psychology 10 (3):287-307.
    Recent experimental findings reveal dissociations of conscious and nonconscious performance in many fields of psychological research, suggesting that conscious and nonconscious effects result from qualitatively different processes. A connectionist view of these processes is put forward in which consciousness is the consequence of construction processes taking place in three types of working memory in a specific type of recurrent neural network. The recurrences arise by feeding back output to the input of a central (representational) network. They are assumed to be (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  3. Stronger suboptimal than optimal affective priming.M. Rotteveel & R. H. Phaf - 2000 - Consciousness and Cognition 9 (2):S66 - S67.
  4.  24
    Mere exposure in reverse: Mood and motion modulate memory bias.Mark Rotteveel & R. Hans Phaf - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1323-1346.
    Mere exposure, generally, entails influences of familiarity manipulations on affective dependent variables. Previously (Phaf & Rotteveel, 2005), we have argued that familiarity corresponds intrinsically to positive affect, and have extended the correspondence to novelty and negative affect. Here, we present two experiments that show reverse effects of affective manipulations on perceived familiarity. In Experiment 1 affectively valenced exteroceptive cues of approach and avoidance (e.g., apparent movement) modulated recognition bias of neutral targets. This finding suggests that our correspondence hypotheses can be (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  5.  33
    Dissociative style and individual differences in verbal working memory span.M. Deruiter, R. Phaf, B. Elzinga & R. Dyck - 2004 - Consciousness and Cognition 13 (4):821-828.
    Dissociative style is mostly studied as a risk factor for dissociative pathology, but it may also reflect a fundamental characteristic of healthy information processing. Due to the close link between attention and working memory and the previous finding of enhanced attentional abilities with a high dissociative style, a positive relationship was also expected between dissociative style and verbal working memory span. In a sample of 119 psychology students, it was found that the verbal span of the high-dissociative group was about (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  6. Frequency and motivational state: evolutionary simulations suggest an adaptive function for network oscillations.Bram T. Heerebout & R. Hans Phaf - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn, Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
  7.  26
    Gamma flicker elicits positive affect without awareness.Bram T. Heerebout, A. E. Yoram Tap, Mark Rotteveel & R. Hans Phaf - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (1):281-289.
    High-frequency oscillations emerged as a neural code for both positive affect and fluent attentional processing from evolutionary simulations with artificial neural networks. Visual 50 Hz flicker, which entrains neural oscillations in the gamma band, has been shown to foster attentional switching, but can it also elicit positive affect? A three-faces display was preceded by a 50, 25, or 0 Hz flicker on the position of the odd-one-out . Participants decided on the gender or on the subjective valence of this neutral (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  8.  25
    A competitive manifesto.R. Hans Phaf & Gezinus Wolters - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (4):487-488.
    The distinction made by Page between localist and distributed representations seems confounded by the distinction between competitive and associative learning. His manifesto can also be read as a plea for competitive learning. The power of competitive models can even be extended further, by simulating similarity effects in forced-choice perceptual identification (Ratcliff & McKoon 1997) that have defied explanation by most memory models.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  17
    Competition elicits arousal and affect.R. Hans Phaf - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39:e220.
    The emotion–cognition integration in Mather et al. can be extended by specifying the relationship between competition and arousal in the reverse direction. According to affective monitoring, competition raises arousal, which, when sustained, results in negative affect, evoking theta oscillations, and when resolved, in positive affect, evoking gamma oscillations. Competition should be considered a core process in both cognition and emotion.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10. Katja Valli, Antti Revonsuo, Outi Pälkäs, Kamaran Hassan Ismahil, Karsan Jelal Ali, and Raija-Leena Punamäki. The.Gayle B. Speck, Kieron P. OÕConnor, Frederick Aardema, Walter J. Perrig, Doris Eckstein, Berenice Valdes Conroy, A. Catena, P. Marı-Beffa, Michiel B. de Ruiter & R. Hans Phaf - 2004 - Consciousness and Cognition 13:655.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  64
    Contrasts and dissociations suggest qualitative differences between conscious and unconscious processes.Gezinus Wolters & R. Hans Phaf - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):359-360.
    The authors reject a computationally powerful unconscious. Instead, they suggest that simple unconscious processes give rise to complex conscious representations. We discuss evidence showing contrastive effects of conscious and unconscious processes, suggesting a distinction between these types of processes. In our view, conscious processes often serve to correct or control negative consequences of relatively simple unconscious processes.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  37
    Constructing consciousness.Gezinus Wolters & R. Hans Phaf - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (1):174-174.
    O'Brien & Opie make unnecessary distinctions between vehicle and process theories and neglect empirically based distinctions between conscious and unconscious processing. We argue that phenomenal experience emerges, not just as a byproduct of input-driven parallel distributed processing, but as a result of constructive processing in recurrent neural networks. Stable network states may be necessary, but are not sufficient, for consciousness.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark