Results for 'negative stimuli'

971 found
Order:
  1.  28
    Freeze or flee? Negative stimuli elicit selective responding.Zachary Estes & Michelle Verges - 2008 - Cognition 108 (2):557-565.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  2.  36
    Predicting facial valence to negative stimuli from resting RSA: Not a function of active emotion regulation.Heath Demaree, Jie Pu, Jennifer Robinson, Brandon Schmeichel & Erik Everhart - 2006 - Cognition and Emotion 20 (2):161-176.
  3.  24
    Retrieval-induced forgetting of negative stimuli: The role of emotional intensity.Christof Kuhbandner, Karl-Heinz Bäuml & Fiona C. Stiedl - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (4):817-830.
  4.  31
    Affective arousal and energization properties of positive and negative stimuli.John Davis & John Lamberth - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (2):196.
  5.  32
    Executive and semantic processes in reappraisal of negative stimuli: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies.Irene Messina, Simone Bianco, Marco Sambin & Roberto Viviani - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  6.  42
    Effects of incidental positive emotion and cognitive reappraisal on affective responses to negative stimuli.Yu Song, Jessica I. Jordan, Kelsey A. Shaffer, Erik K. Wing, Kateri McRae & Christian E. Waugh - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (6):1155-1168.
    ABSTRACTPrevious studies have identified two powerful ways to regulate emotional responses to a stressor: experiencing incidental positive emotions and using cognitive reappraisal to reframe the stressor. Several cognitive and motivational theories of positive emotion support the formulation that incidental positive emotions may facilitate cognitive reappraisal. To test the separate and interacting effects of positive emotions and cognitive reappraisal, we first adapted an established picture-based reappraisal paradigm by interspersing blocks of positive emotion inducing and neutral pictures. Across two pre-registered studies, reappraisal (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  18
    Threat ≠ prevention, challenge ≠ promotion: The impact of threat, challenge and regulatory focus on attention to negative stimuli.Kai Sassenberg, Claudia Sassenrath & Adam K. Fetterman - 2015 - Cognition and Emotion 29 (1):188-195.
  8.  20
    Inhibition of Return Is Modulated by Negative Stimuli: Evidence from Subliminal Perception.Fada Pan, Xiaogang Wu, Li Zhang & Yuhong Ou - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  19
    Does Meditation Alter Brain Responses to Negative Stimuli? A Systematic Review.Andressa A. Magalhaes, Leticia Oliveira, Mirtes G. Pereira & Carolina B. Menezes - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  10.  17
    Both negative and positive task-irrelevant stimuli contract attentional breadth in individuals with high levels of negative affect.Stephanie C. Goodhew & Mark Edwards - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (2):317-331.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  11.  16
    Negative Emotional Stimuli Enhance Conflict Resolution Without Altering Arousal.Daniel J. Fehring, Ranshikha Samandra, Marcello G. Rosa & Farshad A. Mansouri - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
  12.  12
    Stimuli and incentives as determinants of the successive negative contrast effect.James H. McHose - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1 (4):264-266.
  13.  23
    Rule-violations sensitise towards negative and authority-related stimuli.Robert Wirth, Anna Foerster, Hannah Rendel, Wilfried Kunde & Roland Pfister - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (3):480-493.
    Rule violations have usually been studied from a third-person perspective, identifying situational factors that render violations more or less likely. A first-person perspective of the agent that actively violates the rules, on the other hand, is only just beginning to emerge. Here we show that committing a rule violation sensitises towards subsequent negative stimuli as well as subsequent authority-related stimuli. In a Prime-Probe design, we used an instructed rule-violation task as the Prime and a word categorisation task (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  14.  7
    Visual Mismatch Negativity: A Mini-Review of Non-pathological Studies With Special Populations and Stimuli.István Czigler & Petia Kojouharova - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    In this mini-review, we summarized the results of 12 visual mismatch negativity studies that attempted to use this component as a tool for investigating differences between non-clinical samples of participants as well as the possibility of automatic discrimination in the case of specific categories of visual stimuli. These studies investigated the effects of gender, the effects of long-term differences between the groups of participants, and the effects of short-term states, as well as the vMMN effect elicited by artworks as (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  15.  32
    Positive and negative transfer resulting from formal similarity of stimuli.Willard N. Runquist - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (1p1):129.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  12
    Automatic change detection: Mismatch negativity and the now-classic Rensink, O’Reagan, and Clark (1997) stimuli.Domonkos File, Bela Petro, Zsófia Anna Gaál, Nóra Csikós & István Czigler - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Change blindness experiments had demonstrated that detection of significant changes in natural images is extremely difficult when brief blank fields are placed between alternating displays of an original and a modified scene. On the other hand, research on the visual mismatch negativity component of the event-related potentials identified sensitivity to events different from the regularity of stimulus sequences, even if the deviant and standard events are non-attended. The present study sought to investigate the apparent controversy between the experience under the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  15
    Conflict-Elicited Negative Evaluations of Neutral Stimuli: Testing Overt Responses and Stimulus-Frequency Differences as Critical Side Conditions.Florian Goller, Alexandra Kroiss & Ulrich Ansorge - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  18.  35
    The visual mismatch negativity elicited with visual speech stimuli.Benjamin T. Files, Edward T. Auer & Lynne E. Bernstein - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  19.  39
    Neutral but not in the middle: cross-cultural comparisons of negative bias of “neutral” emotional stimuli.Jini Tae, Ye-eun Nam, Yoonhyoung Lee, Rebecca B. Weldon & Myeong-Ho Sohn - 2020 - Cognition and Emotion 34 (6):1171-1182.
    Previous studies have shown that the perception of neutral emotion stimuli can be negative rather than absolutely neutral. In the current study, we examined the negative bias of both neutral faces...
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  20.  63
    Pupillary Response to Negative Emotional Stimuli Is Differentially Affected in Meditation Practitioners.Alejandra Vasquez-Rosati, Enzo P. Brunetti, Carmen Cordero & Pedro E. Maldonado - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
  21.  25
    Memory for negatively arousing and neutral pictorial stimuli using a repeated testing paradigm.Rosalie P. Kern, Terry M. Libkuman & Hajime Otani - 2002 - Cognition and Emotion 16 (6):749-767.
  22.  27
    Automatic attention to stimuli signalling chances and dangers: Moderating effects of positive and negative goal and action contexts.Klaus Rothermund, Dirk Wentura & Peter M. Bak - 2001 - Cognition and Emotion 15 (2):231-248.
  23.  16
    Impact of Distracting Emotional Stimuli on the Characteristics of Movement Performance: A Kinematic Study.Yingzhi Lu, Tianyi Wang, Qiuping Long & Zijian Cheng - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    It is well-documented that emotional stimuli impact both the cognitive and motor aspects of “goal-directed” behavior. However, how emotional distractors impact motor performance remains unclear. This study aimed to characterize how movement quality was impacted during emotional distractors. We used a modified oddball paradigm and documented the performance of pure movement. Participants were designated to draw a triangle or a polygon, while an emotional stimulus was presented. Speed was assessed using reaction time and movement time. The quality and precision (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  42
    Updating in working memory predicts greater emotion reactivity to and facilitated recovery from negative emotion-eliciting stimuli.Madeline L. Pe, Peter Koval, Marlies Houben, Yasemin Erbas, Dominique Champagne & Peter Kuppens - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  25.  31
    The functional field of view becomes narrower while viewing negative emotional stimuli.Tomoe Nobata, Yuji Hakoda & Yuri Ninose - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (5):886-891.
  26. The Role of Emotional Valence for the Processing of Facial and Verbal Stimuli—Positivity or Negativity Bias?Christina Kauschke, Daniela Bahn, Michael Vesker & Gudrun Schwarzer - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  27. Is there an attentional bias to internal stimuli in persons with negative affect.K. Stegen, I. Van Diest, K. P. Van de Woestijne & O. Van den Bergh - 2001 - Cognition Emotion 15:813-29.
  28.  15
    Emotional Empathic Responses to Dynamic Negative Affective Stimuli Is Gender-Dependent.Kim P. C. Kuypers - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29. Focusing on the positive: Age differences in memory for positive, negative, and neutral stimuli.S. T. Charles, M. Mather & L. L. Carstensen - 2003 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 85:163-178.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  30.  46
    Emotional responding in NSSI: examinations of appraisals of positive and negative emotional stimuli, with and without acute stress.Ruth Tatnell, Penelope Hasking, Ottmar V. Lipp, Mark Boyes & Jessica Dawkins - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (6):1304-1316.
    ABSTRACTNon-suicidal self-injury is commonly used by young adults to regulate emotional responses. Yet, experimental examination of how people who self-injure appraise and respond to emotional stimuli is limited. We examined appraisals of, and responses to, emotive images in young adults who did and did not self-injure, and assessed whether these were impacted by exposure to a stressor. Study 1 examined whether participants differed in their appraisals of emotional images. Study 2 assessed whether appraisals of images changed after exposure to (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  47
    Common valence coding in action and evaluation: Affective blindness towards response-compatible stimuli.Andreas B. Eder & Karl Christoph Klauer - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1297-1322.
    A common coding account of bidirectional evaluation–behaviour interactions proposes that evaluative attributes of stimuli and responses are coded in a common representational format. This assumption was tested in two experiments that required evaluations of positive and negative stimuli during the generation of a positively or negatively charged motor response. The results of both experiments revealed a reduced evaluative sensitivity (d′) towards response-compatible stimulus valences. This action–valence blindness supports the notion of a common valence coding in action and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  32.  36
    Epistemic motivation affects the processing of negative emotional stimuli in interpersonal decisions.Zhenyu Wei, María Ruz, Zhiying Zhao & Yong Zheng - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  12
    Broad attention does not buffer the impact of emotionally salient stimuli on performance.Stephanie C. Goodhew & Mark Edwards - 2024 - Cognition and Emotion 38 (3):332-347.
    It has been claimed that a broad attentional breadth buffers the impact of negative stimuli on human perception and cognition. Here we identify issues with the research on which this claim is based, and then rigorously test the claim. To induce narrow versus broad attentional breadth participants attended to the local versus global elements of Navon stimuli, and to investigate the impact of emotionally salient stimuli on performance we measured the effect of task-irrelevant stimuli of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  32
    Auditory scene analysis and sonified visual images. Does consonance negatively impact on object formation when using complex sonified stimuli?David J. Brown, Andrew J. R. Simpson & Michael J. Proulx - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  35.  88
    Behavioral and Brain Reactivity Associated With Drug-Related and Non-Drug-Related Emotional Stimuli in Methamphetamine Addicts.Xiawen Li, Yu Zhou, Guanghui Zhang, Yingzhi Lu, Chenglin Zhou & Hongbiao Wang - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16:894911.
    BackgroundMethamphetamine addicts can experience severe emotional processing disorders, with abnormal responses to emotional and drug-related stimuli. These aberrant behaviors are one of the key factors leading to relapse. Nevertheless, the characteristics of addicts’ responses to drug-related stimuli and their responses to emotional stimuli remain controversial.Methods52 methamphetamine addicts from China passively viewed three different categories of images: Drug-related; positive emotional; and negative emotional. In the first task, participants completed a 9-point Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) scale, rating the valence (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  35
    Effect of negative emotional content on attentional maintenance in working memory.Gaën Plancher, Sarah Massol, Tiphaine Dorel & Hanna Chainay - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (7):1489-1496.
    ABSTRACTPrevious research has shown that emotional stimuli may interfere with working memory processes, but little is known about the process affected. Using a complex span task, the present study investigated the influence of processing negative emotional content on attentional maintenance in WM. In two experiments conducted under articulatory suppression, participants were asked to remember a series of five letters, each of which was followed by an image to be categorised. In half of the trials, the images were (...) and in the other half, they were neutral. In both experiments, our results showed longer processing times for emotional stimuli than neutral stimuli, and lower memory performance when participants processed negative stimuli. We propose that emotional stimuli direct more attentional resources towards the processing component of the WM task, thereby reducing the storage capacity available for the items that are to be remembered. (shrink)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  36
    Are snakes and spiders special? Acquisition of negative valence and modified attentional processing by non-fear-relevant animal stimuli.Helena M. Purkis & Ottmar V. Lipp - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (3):430-452.
  38.  12
    Age-related differences on temporal source memory by using dynamic stimuli: the effects of POV and emotional valence.Adolfo Di Crosta, Pasquale La Malva, Irene Ceccato, Giulia Prete, Nicola Mammarella, Alberto Di Domenico & Rocco Palumbo - 2024 - Cognition and Emotion 38 (7):1114-1121.
    Previous studies have highlighted that temporal source memory can be influenced by factors such as the individual’s age and the emotional valence of the event to be remembered. In this study, we investigated how the different points of view (POVs) from which an event is presented could interact with the relationship between age-related differences and emotional valence on temporal source memory. One hundred and forty-one younger adults (aged 18–30) and 90 older adults (aged 65–74) were presented with a series of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  29
    The effects of emotional stimuli on target detection: Indirect and direct resource costs.Ulrike Ossowski, Sanna Malinen & William S. Helton - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1649-1658.
    The present study was designed to explore the performance costs of negative emotional stimuli in a vigilance task. Forty participants performed a vigilance task in two conditions: one with task-irrelevant negative-arousing pictures and one with task-irrelevant neutral pictures. In addition to performance, we measured subjective state and frontal cerebral activity with near infrared spectroscopy. Overall performance in the negative picture condition was lower than in the neutral picture condition and the negative picture condition had elevated (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  40.  98
    The Childhood Maltreatment Modulates the Impact of Negative Emotional Stimuli on Conflict Resolution.Xianxin Meng, Shuling Gao, Wenwen Liu, Ling Zhang, Tao Suo & Hong Li - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41.  14
    Social identity-based motivation modulates attention bias toward negative information: an event-related brain potential study.Benoit Montalan, Alexis Boitout, Mathieu Veujoz, Arnaud Leleu, Raymonde Germain, Bernard Personnaz, Robert Lalonde & Mohamed Rebaï - 2011 - Socioaffective Neuroscience and Psychology 1:1-15.
    Research has demonstrated that people readily pay more attention to negative than to positive and/or neutral stimuli. However, evidence from recent studies indicated that such an attention bias to negative information is not obligatory but sensitive to various factors. Two experiments using intergroup evaluative tasks (Study 1: a gender-related groups evaluative task and Study 2: a minimal-related groups evaluative task) was conducted to determine whether motivation to strive for a positive social identity - a part of one's (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  36
    The differential similarity of positive and negative information – an affect-induced processing outcome?Hans Alves, Alex Koch & Christian Unkelbach - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (6):1224-1238.
    People judge positive information to be more alike than negative information. This good-bad asymmetry in similarity was argued to constitute a true property of the information ecology (Alves, H., Koch, A., & Unkelbach, C. (2017). Why good is more alike than bad: Processing implications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 69–79). Alternatively, the asymmetry may constitute a processing outcome itself, namely an influence of phasic affect on information processing. Because no research has yet tested whether phasic affect influences perceived similarity (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  43.  28
    Lateralisation of diffuse positive and negative affect: Ascribing valence to ambiguous stimuli.Severine Koch, Rob W. Holland & Ad van Knippenberg - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (3):587-598.
  44.  8
    Empathising with masked targets: limited side effects of face masks on empathy for dynamic, context-rich stimuli.Susanne Scheibe, Felix Grundmann, Bart Kranenborg & Kai Epstude - 2023 - Cognition and Emotion 37 (4):683-695.
    Multiple studies revealed detrimental effects of face masks on communication, including reduced empathic accuracy and enhanced listening effort. Yet, extant research relied on artificial, decontextualised stimuli, which prevented assessing empathy under more ecologically valid conditions. In this preregistered online experiment (N = 272), we used film clips featuring targets reporting autobiographical events to address motivational mechanisms underlying face mask effects on cognitive (empathic accuracy) and emotional facets (emotional congruence, sympathy) of empathy. Surprisingly, targets whose faces were covered by a (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  14
    Visually Perceived Negative Emotion Enhances Mismatch Negativity but Fails to Compensate for Age-Related Impairments.Jiali Chen, Xiaomin Huang, Xianglong Wang, Xuefei Zhang, Sishi Liu, Junqin Ma, Yuanqiu Huang, Anli Tang & Wen Wu - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    Objective: Automatic detection of auditory stimuli, represented by the mismatch negativity, facilitates rapid processing of salient stimuli in the environment. The amplitude of MMN declines with ageing. However, whether automatic detection of auditory stimuli is affected by visually perceived negative emotions with normal ageing remains unclear. We aimed to evaluate how fearful facial expressions affect the MMN amplitude under ageing.Methods: We used a modified oddball paradigm to analyze the amplitude of N100 and MMN in 22 young (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  31
    Corrugator activity confirms immediate negative affect in surprise.Sascha Topolinski & Fritz Strack - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:108172.
    The emotion of surprise entails a complex of immediate responses, such as cognitive interruption, attention allocation to, and more systematic processing of the surprising stimulus. All these processes serve the ultimate function to increase processing depth and thus cognitively master the surprising stimulus. The present account introduces phasic negative affect as the underlying mechanism responsible for this switch in operating mode. Surprising stimuli are schema-discrepant and thus entail cognitive disfluency, which elicits immediate negative affect. This affect in (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  47.  40
    Coupled positive and negative feedback circuits form an essential building block of cellular signaling pathways.Dongsan Kim, Yung-Keun Kwon & Kwang-Hyun Cho - 2007 - Bioessays 29 (1):85-90.
    Cellular circuits have positive and negative feedback loops that allow them to respond properly to noisy external stimuli. It is intriguing that such feedback loops exist in many cases in a particular form of coupled positive and negative feedback loops with different time delays. As a result of our mathematical simulations and investigations into various experimental evidences, we found that such coupled feedback circuits can rapidly turn on a reaction to a proper stimulus, robustly maintain its status, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  48.  54
    Recovering from negative events by boosting implicit positive affect.Markus Quirin, Regina C. Bode & Julius Kuhl - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (3):559-570.
    Upregulation of implicit positive affect (PA) can act as a mechanism to deal with negative affect. Two studies tracked temporal changes in positive and negative affect (NA) assessed by self-report and the Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (IPANAT; Quirin, Kazén, & Kuhl, 2009). Study 1 observed the predicted increases in implicit PA after exposure to a threat-related film clip, which correlated positively with the speed of recognising a happy face among an angry crowd. Study 2 replicated (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  49.  19
    The effect of fear-inducing stimuli on risk taking in people with psychopathic traits.Angela Book, Beth Visser & Tori Wattam - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (7):1313-1326.
    Research suggests that people with psychopathic traits experience fear-inducing stimuli differently from others, seeming to interpret fear as more positive and less negative. We expected that this reaction, termed fear enjoyment, would impact the effect of fear-inducing stimuli on self-report risk-taking behaviour. Risk-taking was measured before and after viewing excitement- and fear-inducing videos (N = 825). As expected from research showing that fear induction tends to reduce risk-taking tendencies, participants showed reduced risk-taking scores following a fear-inducing stimulus. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  55
    Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology.John R. Hibbing, Kevin B. Smith & John R. Alford - 2014 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37 (3):297-307.
    Disputes between those holding differing political views are ubiquitous and deep-seated, and they often follow common, recognizable lines. The supporters of tradition and stability, sometimes referred to as conservatives, do battle with the supporters of innovation and reform, sometimes referred to as liberals. Understanding the correlates of those distinct political orientations is probably a prerequisite for managing political disputes, which are a source of social conflict that can lead to frustration and even bloodshed. A rapidly growing body of empirical evidence (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
1 — 50 / 971