Results for 'visual distance'

977 found
Order:
  1.  30
    Visual distance discrimination in the rat.Ann Greenhut - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 47 (3):148.
  2.  18
    Accuracy of absolute visual distance and size estimation in space as a function of stereopsis and motion parallax.James W. Dees - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (3):466.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  23
    Formulas for visual distance and size; their relationship to the Nernst-Hill theory of nervous excitation.H. Spotnitz - 1938 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 23 (4):394.
  4.  15
    Auditory and visual distance estimation after active or passive visual deprivation.Robert I. Reynolds - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (3):237-238.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  21
    Is something out of reach more attractive? The effectiveness of visual distance in computational advertising.Tong Liu & Zhengdong Yu - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    With the development of mobile Internet technology, firms need to complete the entire process of consumer targeting, ad content generation, and ad display in a very short time window. Therefore, computational advertising, such as native ads on social media platforms, has become the mainstream of online advertising with its automation and personalization features. However, computational advertising faces some problems when using artificial intelligence technology to generate content. First, the images should have a significant enough impact on consumers and be easy (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  17
    Time, distance, and feature trade-offs in visual apparent motion.Peter Burt & George Sperling - 1981 - Psychological Review 88 (2):171-195.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  7. Long-distance feedback projections to area v1: Implications for multisensory integration, spatial awareness, and visual consciousness.Simon Clavagnier, Arnaud Falchier & Henry Kennedy - 2004 - Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience. Special Issue 4 (2):117-126.
  8.  31
    Visual perception and regulatory conflict: Motivation and physiology influence distance perception.Shana Cole, Emily Balcetis & Sam Zhang - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142 (1):18.
  9.  29
    Distance perception as a function of available visual cues.Teodor Kunnapas - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (4):523.
  10.  22
    Visual acuity and distance of observation.J. G. Beebe-Center, L. C. Mead, K. S. Wagoner & A. C. Hoffman - 1945 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 35 (6):473.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  19
    (1 other version)Visual target distance, but not visual cursor path length produces shifts in motor behavior: a multisensory integration perspective.Loes C. J. Van Dam - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  25
    Visual perspective, distance, and felt presence of others in dreams.Burak Erdeniz, Ege Tekgün, Bigna Lenggenhager & Christophe Lopez - 2023 - Consciousness and Cognition 113 (C):103547.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  73
    How Distractor Objects Trigger Referential Overspecification: Testing the Effects of Visual Clutter and Distractor Distance.Ruud Koolen, Emiel Krahmer & Marc Swerts - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (7):1617-1647.
    In two experiments, we investigate to what extent various visual saliency cues in realistic visual scenes cause speakers to overspecify their definite object descriptions with a redundant color attribute. The results of the first experiment demonstrate that speakers are more likely to redundantly mention color when visual clutter is present in a scene as compared to when this is not the case. In the second experiment, we found that distractor type and distractor color affect redundant color use: (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  14.  23
    Handedness and adaptation to visual distortions of size and distance.S. M. Luria, Christine L. McKay & Steven H. Ferris - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 100 (2):263.
  15.  23
    Bilingual representation of distance in visual-verbal sign systems: A case study of Guo Xi’s Early Spring.Chengzhi Jiang - 2018 - Semiotica 2018 (222):47-80.
    Journal Name: Semiotica Issue: Ahead of print.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  29
    Perceived size and distance in visual space.Alberta S. Gilinsky - 1951 - Psychological Review 58 (6):460-482.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  17.  26
    Distance Learning Methods in Continuing Education of Paramedics.Piotr Leszczyński, Anna Charuta, Joanna Gotlib, Barbara Kołodziejczak, Magdalena Roszak & Tamara Zacharuk - 2017 - Studies in Logic, Grammar and Rhetoric 51 (1):53-70.
    The process of continuing education of paramedics is based on gaining educational credits during five-year educational periods. One of the forms of self-improvement are Internet-based educational programs. The lack of regulations concerning the organizational and technical aspects of e-learning made the authors attempt to analyze the phenomenon. The aim of the article is to present an initial analysis of the role of online educational programs in comparison with other forms of professional training of paramedics. One in three respondents has recently (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  18.  34
    Distance judgment by the method of fractionation.Jean Purdy & Eleanor J. Gibson - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 50 (6):374.
  19.  21
    The role of perceived distance in determining apparent visual size.Leonard Brosgole, Thomas J. PlaHovinsak, Miguel Roig & Joseph P. Notaro - 1985 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 23 (6):489-492.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  17
    Voluntary control of the distance location of the visual field.Harvey Carr - 1908 - Psychological Review 15 (3):139-149.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  30
    Relation of the narrowing of the visual field with an increase in distance to manifest anxiety.Harald-Edwin Schmidt - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (4):334.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  13
    Gilinsky's theory of visual size and distance.William M. Smith - 1952 - Psychological Review 59 (3):239-243.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  66
    Distance and Direction in Reid’s Theory of Vision.Giovanni B. Grandi - 2016 - Topoi 35 (2):465-478.
    Two theses appear to be central to Reid’s view of the visual field. By sight, we do not originally perceive depth or linear distance from the eye. By sight, we originally perceive the position that points on the surface of objects have with regard to the centre of the eye. In different terms, by sight, we originally perceive the compass direction and degree of elevation of points on the surface of objects with reference to the centre of the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  66
    Long-Distance Caring Labor.Rob Spicer - 2014 - Techné: Research in Philosophy and Technology 18 (1/2):102-116.
    This article is an exploration of Apple’s iPhone 4 as both a technology and an object of marketing. This analysis looks at the FaceTime app and how its marketing created a visual hand-phone-face Deleuzian assemblage while playing on affective connections of parenthood and long-distance caring labor. This is connected to the ways in which mobile telephony creates divided attention between home and labor and the mobile phone and car while driving. This analysis is especially concerned with technological transparency (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  50
    8 Entry and Distance: Sublimity in Landscape.Andrew Benjamin - 2011 - In Jeff Malpas (ed.), The Place of Landscape: Concepts, Contexts, Studies. MIT Press. pp. 151.
    This chapter focuses on a 1914 photograph by Stefano Bricarelli that can be considered a visual representation of the concerns of landscape. It explores the concept of the sublime in terms of the interplay between distance, immediacy, and representation. Before considering the concept of sublimity, the question regarding entry into the scene must first be addressed. The possibility that landscape may only exist because of such an entry plays a vital role in any analysis of the photographic image. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  17
    Does intraocular straylight predict night driving visual performance? Correlations between straylight levels and contrast sensitivity, halo size, and hazard recognition distance with and without glare.Judith Ungewiss, Ulrich Schiefer, Peter Eichinger, Michael Wörner, David P. Crabb & Pete R. Jones - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16:910620.
    PurposeTo evaluate the relationship between intraocular straylight perception and: (i) contrast sensitivity (CS), (ii) halo size, and (iii) hazard recognition distance, in the presence and absence of glare.Subjects and methodsParticipants were 15 (5 female) ophthalmologically healthy adults, aged 54.6–80.6 (median: 67.2) years. Intraocular straylight (log s) was measured using a straylight meter (C-Quant; Oculus GmbH, Wetzlar, Germany). CS with glare was measured clinically using the Optovist I device (Vistec Inc., Olching, Germany) and also within a driving simulator using Landolt (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  14
    Interpreting Estonian Demonstratives: The Effects of Referent’s Distance and Visual Salience.Maria Reile, Kristiina Averin & Nele Põldver - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Most of the research done with spatial demonstratives have focused on the production, not the interpretation, of these words. In addition, emphasis has been largely on demonstrative pronouns, leaving demonstrative adverbs with relatively little research attention. The present study explores the interpretation of both demonstrative pronouns and demonstrative adverbs in Estonian—a Finno-Ugric language with two dialectal-specific demonstrative pronoun systems. In the South-Estonian dialectal region, two demonstrative pronouns, see—“this” and too—“that”, are used. In the North-Estonian region, only one, see—“this/that”, is used. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  58
    Vanquishing Temporal Distance: Malraux, Art and Metamorphosis.Derek Allan - 2016 - Australian Journal of French Studies 53 (1-2):136-148.
    How does art – literature, visual art, or music – endure over time? What special power does it possess that enables it to “transcend” time – to overcome temporal distance and speak to us not just as evidence of times gone by, but as a living presence? The Renaissance, which discovered this transcendent power of art in the classical sculpture and literature it admired so strongly, concluded that great art is impervious to time – “timeless”, “immortal”, “eternal” – (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  30
    Blindsight in rodents: The use of a "high-level" distance cue in gerbils with lesions of primary visual cortex.D. P. Carey, Melvyn A. Goodale & E. G. Sprowl - 1990 - Behavioural Brain Research 38:283-289.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  30. Relative effectiveness of size and distance cues in visual-attention.J. F. Juola, E. Cooper & B. Warner - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (5):349-349.
  31.  18
    Gaze and the Eye Pupil Adjust to Imagined Size and Distance.Unni Sulutvedt, Thea K. Mannix & Bruno Laeng - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (8):3159-3176.
    Pupillary responses and associated vergence eye movements were monitored during imagery of objects of differing sizes (“large” or “small”) from varying distances (“near” or “far”). Objects’ imagined size and distance affected oculomotor behavior. Objects visualized as “far” resulted in the larger pupil dilations and smaller visual angle, while small objects imagined “near” were associated with smaller pupils in contrast to relatively larger pupils when imagined as “far” away. Furthermore, near objects resulted in larger visual angle, and particularly, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  32.  13
    Citizen Environmental Behavior From the Perspective of Psychological Distance Based on a Visual Analysis of Bibliometrics and Scientific Knowledge Mapping.Li Yang, Xin Fang & Junqi Zhu - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Global warming and other climate issues seriously threaten global sustainable development. As citizen environmental behavior can have a positive impact on the environment, it is of great theoretical significance and practical reference value to study the impact of psychological distance theory on citizen environmental behavior. This study obtained 2,633 related studies from 1980 to 2020 from the Web of Science as research objects, and used CiteSpace, VOSviewer, Netdraw, and other software programs to perform a bibliometric analysis, which can show (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  54
    On Natural Geometry and Seeing Distance Directly in Descartes.Gary Hatfield - 2015 - In Vincenzo De Risi (ed.), Mathematizing Space: The Objects of Geometry from Antiquity to the Early Modern Age. Birkhäuser. pp. 157-91.
    As the word “optics” was understood from antiquity into and beyond the early modern period, it did not mean simply the physics and geometry of light, but meant the “theory of vision” and included what we should now call physiological and psychological aspects. From antiquity, these aspects were subject to geometrical analysis. Accordingly, the geometry of visual experience has long been an object of investigation. This chapter examines accounts of size and distance perception in antiquity (Euclid and Ptolemy) (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  34.  19
    Is scaling up harder than scaling down? How children and adults visually scale distance from memory.Jodie M. Plumert, Alycia M. Hund & Kara M. Recker - 2019 - Cognition 185 (C):39-48.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  12
    Neural correlates of distorted body representations underlying tactile distance perception.Luigi Tamè, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Renata Sadibolova, Martin I. Sereno & Matthew R. Longo - unknown
    Tactile distance perception is believed to require that immediate afferent signals be referenced to a stored representation of body size and shape (the body model). For this ability, recent studies have reported that the stored body representations involved are highly distorted, at least in the case of the hand, with the hand dorsum represented as wider and squatter than it actually is. Here, we aim to define the neural basis of this phenomenon. In a behavioural experiment participants estimated the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  27
    The effectiveness of size cues to relative distance as a function of lateral visual separation.Walter C. Gogel & George S. Harker - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 50 (5):309.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  45
    Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology. Visual Peception of Shape, Space and Appearance.Liliana Albertazzi (ed.) - 2013 - Chichester, West Sussex, UK: Wiley.
    Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance Liliana Albertazzi. the sort I have in mind. What I am speaking of is the mandatory correlations between attributes of visual space (those of, e.g., surfaces, shape, distance, direction) and  ...
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  38. Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: Young children’s use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task.Sang Ah Lee, Valeria A. Sovrano & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2012 - Cognition 123 (1):144-161.
  39.  47
    Memory and Distance: Learning from a Gilded Silver Vase (Antwerp, c. 1530).Carlo Ginzburg - 2004 - Diogenes 51 (1):99-112.
    This article concerns a silver beaker (now at the Residenzmuseum, Munich) decorated with scenes which seem to be related to the Spanish conquest of Mexico. On the basis of stylistic, iconographic and archival evidence the silversmith is here tentatively identified with an Italian-born artist, Stefano Capello, who is thought to have added a decoration to a pre-existing beaker on the eve of the treaty of Cambrai (3 August 1529). Margaret of Austria, aunt of the emperor Charles V, might have given (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40. Immersive ideals / critical distances : study of the affinity between artistic ideologies in virtual Reality and previous immersive idioms.Joseph Nechvatal (ed.) - 2010 - Berlin: LAP Lambert Academic Publishing AG & Co KG.
    My research into Virtual Reality technology and its central property of immersion has indicated that immersion in Virtual Reality (VR) electronic systems is a significant key to the understanding of contemporary culture as well as considerable aspects of previous culture as detected in the histories of philosophy and the visual arts. The fundamental change in aesthetic perception engendered by immersion, a perception which is connected to the ideal of total-immersion in virtual space, identifies certain shifts in ontology which are (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  13
    The Distant: Thinking toward Renewed Senses of Landscape and Distance.John Wylie - 2017 - Environment, Space, Place 9 (1):1-20.
    Abstract:There is an established narrative in which the world shrinks, distances are overcome and rendered insignificant, and the near and the far lose their salience as means of orientation and understanding. Yet, within this narrative, new distances are equally felt and observed to have opened up. New distances between and amongst us, multiplying distances of indifference, incomprehension and antagonism. And felt distances also between ourselves and ‘land’ and ‘nature’—a sense of separation, alienation and loss which it then becomes imperative, ethically (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  20
    The Organization of a Foreign Language Distance Learning in Quarantine During the Postmodern Era.Roksolana Povoroznyuk, Nataliia Tonkonoh, Iryna Berezneva, Yuriy Sobkov, Olha Trebyk & Alla Gembaruk - 2022 - Postmodern Openings 13 (1 Sup1):494-508.
    The article addresses organization of distance learning of foreign languages in quarantine during the postmodern era. It is noted that organization of distance learning in the educational process is very important. It was clarified that distance learning is based on synchronous and asynchronous modes. The essence of the concepts of “distance learning”, “asynchronous learning”, “synchronous learning”, “quarantine” is explained. Articles of domestic and foreign teachers and scientists analysing the broached subject were reviewed. The article highlights advantages (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  14
    On Visually-Grounded Reference Production: Testing the Effects of Perceptual Grouping and 2D/3D Presentation Mode.Ruud Koolen - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:470418.
    When referring to a target object in a visual scene, speakers are assumed to consider certain distractor objects to be more relevant than others. The current research predicts that the way in which speakers come to a set of relevant distractors depends on how they perceive the distance between the objects in the scene. It reports on the results of two language production experiments, in which participants referred to target objects in photo-realistic visual scenes. Experiment 1 manipulated (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  44.  42
    Four Ways of Measuring the Distance between Alchemy and Contemporary Art.James Elkins - 2003 - Hyle 9 (1):105 - 118.
    Alchemy has always had its ferocious defenders, and a small minority of artists remain interested in alchemical meanings and substances. In this essay I will suggest two reasons why alchemy is marginal to current visual art, and two more reasons why alchemical thinking remains absolutely central. Briefly: alchemy is irrelevant because (1) it is has been a minority interest from early modernism to the present, and therefore (2) it is outside the principal conversations about modernism and postmodernism; but alchemy (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  45.  27
    Plagiarism Intervention Using a Game-Based Tutorial in an Online Distance Education Course.Cheryl A. Kier - 2019 - Journal of Academic Ethics 17 (4):429-439.
    This project assesses the ability of a game tutorial, “Goblin Threat” to increase university students’ ability to recognize plagiarized passages. The game tutorial covers information about how to cite properly, types and consequences of plagiarism, and the differences between paraphrasing and plagiarism. The game involves finding and clicking on “goblins” who ask questions about various aspects of plagiarism. Sound effects and entertaining visuals work to keep students’ attention. One group of 177 students enrolled in an online Psychology of Adolescence course (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  46. Visual features as carriers of abstract quantitative information.Ronald A. Rensink - 2022 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 8 (151):1793-1820.
    Four experiments investigated the extent to which abstract quantitative information can be conveyed by basic visual features. This was done by asking observers to estimate and discriminate Pearson correlation in graphical representations where the first data dimension of each element was encoded by its horizontal position, and the second by the value of one of its visual features; perceiving correlation then requires combining the information in the two encodings via a common abstract representation. Four visual features were (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. How Infants Learn About the Visual World.Scott P. Johnson - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (7):1158-1184.
    The visual world of adults consists of objects at various distances, partly occluding one another, substantial and stable across space and time. The visual world of young infants, in contrast, is often fragmented and unstable, consisting not of coherent objects but rather surfaces that move in unpredictable ways. Evidence from computational modeling and from experiments with human infants highlights three kinds of learning that contribute to infants’ knowledge of the visual world: learning via association, learning via active (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  48. Representation and constraints: The inverse problem and the structure of visual space.Gary Hatfield - 2003 - Acta Psychologica 114:355-378.
    Visual space can be distinguished from physical space. The first is found in visual experience, while the second is defined independently of perception. Theorists have wondered about the relation between the two. Some investigators have concluded that visual space is non-Euclidean, and that it does not have a single metric structure. Here it is argued that visual space exhibits contraction in all three dimensions with increasing distance from the observer, that experienced features of this contraction (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  49.  52
    Two visual hallucinatory syndromes.Dominic H. Ffytche - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (6):763-764.
    When viewed from a distance, visual hallucinations fall into one of two symptom patterns, a dichotomy which poses a problem for theoretical models treating them as a single entity. Such models should be broadened to allow for two distinct but overlapping syndromes – one likely to relate to visual de-afferentation, the other to Perception and Attention Deficit (PAD) cholinergic pathology.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  43
    The Dimensionality of Visual Space.William H. Rosar - 2016 - Topoi 35 (2):531-570.
    The empirical study of visual space has centered on determining its geometry, whether it is a perspective projection, flat or curved, Euclidean or non-Euclidean, whereas the topology of space consists of those properties that remain invariant under stretching but not tearing. For that reason distance is a property not preserved in topological space whereas the property of spatial order is preserved. Specifically the topological properties of dimensionality, orientability, continuity, and connectivity define “real” space as studied by physics and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 977