Results for ' DISCRIMINATION, STIMULUS FUNCTION OF'

982 found
Order:
  1.  37
    Stimulus generalization of a positive conditioned reinforcer: IV. Concurrent generalization of reinforcing and discriminative stimulus functions following fixed-interval training.David R. Thomas & Donald V. Derosa - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (2):260.
  2.  33
    A quantitative comparison of the discriminative and reinforcing functions of a stimulus.James A. Dinsmoor - 1950 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 40 (4):458.
  3.  37
    Reaction time as a function of perceptual bias, response bias, and stimulus discriminability.Howard B. Orenstein - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 86 (1):38.
  4.  36
    Relation between stimulus intensity and operant response rate as a function of discrimination training and drive.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (1):9.
  5.  23
    Stimulus generalization along the dimension of S+ as a function of discrimination learning with and without error.Joseph Lyons - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (1):95.
  6.  15
    Stimulus control in the albino rat as a function of extradimensional discrimination training.Joseph Lyons, Janet D. Lyons & William D. Klipec - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1 (1):9-10.
  7.  25
    Primary stimulus generalization in discrimination learning as a function of number of trials and incidental cue differences.Leopold O. Walder - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (2):178.
  8.  32
    Two-choice discrimination learning as a function of stimulus and event probabilities.Jerome L. Myers & Donna Cruse - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (3p1):453.
  9.  18
    Pitch discrimination as a function of tonal duration.W. W. Turnbull - 1944 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 34 (4):302.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  46
    The span of visual discrimination as a function of time and intensity of stimulation.W. S. Hunter & M. Sigler - 1940 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 26 (2):160.
  11.  28
    Differential eyelid conditioning as a function of stimulus similarity and strength of response to the CS.Malcolm D. Gynther - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (6):408.
  12.  32
    Stimulus familiarity modulates functional connectivity of the perirhinal cortex and anterior hippocampus during visual discrimination of faces and objects.Victoria C. McLelland, David Chan, Susanne Ferber & Morgan D. Barense - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  13.  46
    Secondary reinforcement in rats as a function of information value and reliability of the stimulus.M. David Egger & Neal E. Miller - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (2):97.
  14.  20
    Extinction of the human eyelid CR as a function of the discriminability of the change from acquisition to extinction.Kenneth W. Spence, M. J. Homzie & E. F. Rutledge - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (6):545.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  40
    Chronic effects of a high-frequency stimulus on the structure and function of the cochlea.Irving E. Alexander & Frederick J. Githler - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (5):363.
  16.  21
    Perception as a function of association value with response bias controlled.Stephanie Portnoy, Maurice Portnoy & Kurt Salzinger - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (3):316.
  17.  23
    Simple concept learning as a function of intralist generalization.Marian Hooper Baum - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 47 (2):89.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  27
    Symbolic Processes and Stimulus Equivalence.Ullin T. Place - 1995 - Behavior and Philosophy 23 (3-1):13 - 30.
    A symbol is defined as a species of sign. The concept of a sign coincides with Skinner's (1938) concept of a discriminative stimulus. Symbols differ from other signs in five respects: (1) They are stimuli which the organism can both respond to and produce, either as a self-directed stimulus (as in thinking) or as a stimulus for another individual with a predictably similar response from the recipient in each case. (2) they act as discriminative stimuli for the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  19.  44
    Stimulus configuration, long-term potentiation, and the hippocampus.Nestor A. Schmajuk - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):629-631.
    Shors & Matzel propose that hippocampal LTP increases the effective salience of discrete external stimuli and thereby facilitates the induction of memories at distant places. In line with this suggestion, a neural network model of associative learning and hippocampal function assumes that LTP increases hippocampal error signals to the cortex, thereby facilitating stimulus configuration in association cortex. Computer simulations show that under these assumptions the model correctly describes the effect of LTP induction and blockade in classical discriminations and (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  33
    Temporal dynamics of task switching and abstract-concept learning in pigeons.Thomas A. Daniel, Robert G. Cook & Jeffrey S. Katz - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:158480.
    The current study examined whether pigeons could learn to use abstract concepts as the basis for conditionally switching behavior as a function of time. Using a mid-session reversal task, experienced pigeons were trained to switch from matching-to-sample (MTS) to non-matching-to-sample (NMTS) conditional discriminations within a session. One group had prior training with MTS, while the other had prior training with NMTS. Over training, stimulus set size was progressively doubled from 3 to 6 to 12 stimuli to promote abstract (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  21
    Stimulus determinants of choice behavior in visual pattern discrimination.Jaques Kaswan, Stephen Young & Charles Y. Nakamura - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (5):441.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  14
    The functionality of the principle of non-discrimination on grounds of gender, race, religion and sexual orientation in the postmodern society.Oleg SPÎNU - 2021 - Postmodern Openings 12 (2).
    Discrimination in the postmodern society can have many different causes and can affect people of different racial, ethnic, national or social backgrounds, such as communities of Asian or African descent, Roma people, indigenous peoples, Aboriginal people and people of different castes. Discrimination can also refer to people of different cultural, linguistic or religious backgrounds, people with disabilities or the elderly. Moreover, people can be discriminated because of their sexual orientation or preferences. Gender-based discrimination is also common, despite progress in many (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  27
    Stimulus generalization of a positive conditioned reinforcer: II. Effects of discrimination training.David R. Thomas & Salvatore C. Caronite - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (4):402.
  24.  28
    Stimulus generalization of an instrumental response as a function of the number of reinforced trials.Garry Margolius - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (2):105.
  25.  23
    Stimulus correlates of visual pattern discrimination by humans: Area and contour.V. J. Polidora - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (3):221.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  21
    Memory span as a function of variable presentation speeds and stimulus durations.M. C. Corballis - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (3):461.
  27.  98
    Behavioral momentum and the law of effect.John A. Nevin & Randolph C. Grace - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (1):73-90.
    In the metaphor of behavioral momentum, the rate of a free operant in the presence of a discriminative stimulus is analogous to the velocity of a moving body, and resistance to change measures an aspect of behavior that is analogous to its inertial mass. An extension of the metaphor suggests that preference measures an analog to the gravitational mass of that body. The independent functions relating resistance to change and preference to the conditions of reinforcement may be construed as (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  28.  26
    Concept acquisition as a function of stimulus-equivalence pretraining with identical and dissimilar stimuli.Richard D. Petre - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (4):360.
  29.  18
    Stimulus generalization as a function of contextual stimuli.Harry Helson & Lloyd L. Avant - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 73 (4p1):565.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  34
    Choice response times as functions of intralist similarity, stimulus type, and number of equally probable alternatives.Barry Gholson & Raymond H. Hohle - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (3):581.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  31
    Frequency discrimination as a function of frequency of repetition and trials.Robert C. Radtke, Larry L. Jacoby & George D. Goedel - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 89 (1):78.
  32.  24
    Differential conditioning as a function of exposure time to discriminative and nondiscriminative cues preceding response.Stephen F. Davis & H. Wayne Ludvigson - 1974 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (4):385-388.
  33.  47
    Stimulus generalization as a function of the serial position of the stimulus during prior training.Marvin E. Shaw & F. A. King - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 52 (4):228.
  34.  23
    The effect of a discriminative stimulus transferred to a previously unassociated response.K. C. Walker - 1942 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 31 (4):312.
  35.  56
    Distributed functions of detection and discrimination of vibrotactile stimuli in the hierarchical human somatosensory system.Junsuk Kim, Klaus-Robert Mã¼Ller, Yoon Gi Chung, Soon-Cheol Chung, Jang-Yeon Park, Heinrich H. Bã¼Lthoff & Sung-Phil Kim - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  36.  21
    Stimulus generalization as a function of drive shift.Robert B. Zajonc & David V. Cross - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (4):363.
  37.  28
    Conditioned fear as a function of CS-UCS and probe stimulus intervals.Leonard E. Ross - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (4):265.
  38.  22
    Habit strength as a function of drive in a brightness discrimination problem.Eugene Eisman, Adele Asimow & Irving Maltzman - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 52 (1):58.
  39.  11
    Stimulus facilitation of delayed-reward performance as a function of the cue’s spatial position.Roger M. Tarpy & Frederick L. Sawabini - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 6 (4):385-387.
  40.  35
    Retroaction as a function of discrimination and motor variables.M. L. Ritchie & F. A. Muckler - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (6):409.
  41.  24
    Human vigilance as a function of signal frequency and stimulus density.William A. Johnston, William C. Howell & Irwin L. Goldstein - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (5):736.
  42.  35
    Absolute judgments as a function of stimulus range and number of stimulus and response categories.Charles W. Eriksen & Harold W. Hake - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (5):323.
  43.  25
    Stimulus generalization as a function of UCS intensity in eyelid conditioning.John J. Porter - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (3):311.
  44.  25
    Discrimination learning as a function of reversal and nonreversal shifts.Roger T. Kelleher - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (6):379.
  45.  14
    Discrimination learning as a function of varying pairs of sucrose rewards.Roger W. Black - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (5):452.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  66
    Neuroethology of releasing mechanisms: Prey-catching in toads.Jörg-Peter Ewert - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):337-368.
    Abstract“Sign stimuli” elicit specific patterns of behavior when an organism's motivation is appropriate. In the toad, visually released prey-catching involves orienting toward the prey, approaching, fixating, and snapping. For these action patterns to be selected and released, the prey must be recognized and localized in space. Toads discriminate prey from nonprey by certain spatiotemporal stimulus features. The stimulus-response relations are mediated by innate releasing mechanisms (RMs) with recognition properties partly modifiable by experience. Striato-pretecto-tectal connectivity determines the RM's recognition (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   97 citations  
  47.  18
    Fixation time as a function of stimulus uncertainty.James Allison - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (4):433.
  48.  39
    Stimulus generalization as a function of drive level, and the relation between two measures of response strength.J. Robert Newman & G. Robert Grice - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (4):357.
  49.  22
    Stimulus generalization as a function of verbal reinforcement combination.Arnold H. Buss, Morton Weiner & Edith Buss - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (6):433.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  19
    Incentive preference as a function of water deprivation and locus of discriminative stimuli.Jerome S. Cohen, Anke Oöstendorp & William Ross - 1977 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 9 (6):387-390.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 982