Results for ' stimulus information'

974 found
Order:
  1.  62
    Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time.Ray Hyman - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 45 (3):188.
  2.  36
    Stimulus information and contextual information as determinants of tachistoscopic recognition of words.Endel Tulving & Cecille Gold - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (4):319.
  3.  16
    Interaction between total stimulus information and specific stimulus information in visual recognition.J. R. Newbrough - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (3):297.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  29
    Effects of stimulus information reduction on search time of retarded adolescents and normal children.Herman H. Spitz - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (3):482.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  21
    Reduction of redundant stimulus information in short-term memory.Robert E. Morin, Dorothy S. Konick & Kenneth L. Hoving - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 2 (1):29-30.
  6.  19
    Reaction time as a function of stimulus information and age.George J. Suci, Melvin D. Davidoff & Walter W. Surwillo - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (4):242.
  7.  33
    Conceptual distortions of hand structure are robust to changes in stimulus information.Klaudia B. Ambroziak, Luigi Tamè & Matthew R. Longo - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 61:107-116.
    Hands are commonly held up as an exemplar of well-known, familiar objects. However, conceptual knowledge of the hand has been found to show highly stereotyped distortions. Specifically, people judge their knuckles as farther forward in the hand than they actually are. The cause of this distal bias remains unclear. In Experiment 1, we tested whether both visual and tactile information contribute to the distortion. Participants judged the location of their knuckles by pointing to the location on their palm directly (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  40
    Automatic and attention-dependent processing of auditory stimulus information.Risto Näätänen - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):261-288.
  9.  21
    Search-discrimination time for missing stimulus information.Aley Thomas & Charles M. Solley - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (5):501.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  25
    Information processing behavior: The role of irrelevant stimulus information.Robert E. Morin, Bert Forrin & Wayne Archer - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (1):89.
  11.  69
    Contingencies and attentional capture: the importance of matching stimulus informativeness in the item-specific proportion congruent task.James R. Schmidt - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
  12.  29
    Information theory and stimulus encoding in free and serial recall: Ordinal position of formal similarity.Douglas L. Nelson - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 80 (3p1):537.
  13.  14
    Stimulus-response coding and amount of information as determinants of reaction time.Sidney Hellyer - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (5):521.
  14.  29
    Information and incentive value of the reinforcing stimulus in verbal conditioning.Charles D. Spielberger, Ira H. Bernstein & Richard G. Ratliff - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (1):26.
  15.  31
    Information theory and stimulus encoding in paired-associate acquisition: Ordinal position of formal similarity.Douglas L. Nelson & Frank A. Rowe - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 79 (2p1):342.
  16.  62
    Information: In the stimulus or in the context?Giulio Tononi & Gerald M. Edelman - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):698-700.
    The distinction between receptive field and conceptual field is appealing and heuristically useful. Conceptually, it is more satisfactory to distinguish between information from the environment and from the brain. We emphasize here a selectionist view that considers information transmission within the brain as modulated by a stimulus, rather than information transmission from a stimulus as modulated by the context.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  29
    Information value and stimulus configuring as factors in conditioned reinforcement.David R. Thomas, David L. Berman & George E. Serednesky - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 76 (2p1):181.
  18.  38
    Choices based on redundant information: An analysis of two-dimensional stimulus control.Sheila Chase & Eric G. Heinemann - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (2):161.
  19.  20
    Stimulus-determined and perceiver-determined aspects of haptic perceptual information processing.Paul J. Locher - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (3):197-200.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  33
    Effect of stimulus condition and reaction time information on spatial stimulus generalization.Charles Y. Nakamura & Jaques W. Kaswan - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (1):67.
  21. The role of stimulus-based and response-based spatial information in sequence learning.Koch Iring & Hoffmann Joachim - 2000 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 26 (4).
  22.  38
    Instructions and stimulus properties in processing relevant-redundant information.Herman Staudenmayer & Roger W. Schvaneveldt - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 89 (1):175.
  23.  16
    Information processing of olfactory stimuli by the dog: II. Stimulus control and sampling strategies in simultaneous discrimination learning.R. E. Lubow, Moshe Kahn & Reuven Frommer - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 8 (4):323-326.
  24.  27
    Cognitive aspects of information processing: II. Adjustments to stimulus redundancy.Paul M. Fitts, James R. Peterson & Gerson Wolpe - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (5):423.
  25.  16
    Stimulus size and acuity in information processing.Derek W. Schultz & Charles W. Eriksen - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 12 (6):397-399.
  26.  45
    Secondary reinforcement in rats as a function of information value and reliability of the stimulus.M. David Egger & Neal E. Miller - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (2):97.
  27.  18
    Effects of stimulus probability and information feedback on response biases in children’s recognition memory.Daniel B. Berch - 1977 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 10 (4):328-330.
  28.  34
    Effects of stimulus alternation on sequential information processing by retarded and nonretarded subjects.Edward A. Holden - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 101 (2):262.
  29.  30
    On the heterogeneity of stimulus and response elements in the processing of information.William C. Howell - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (3):235.
  30.  53
    Uncovering the Richness of the Stimulus: Structure Dependence and Indirect Statistical Evidence.Florencia Reali & Morten H. Christiansen - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (6):1007-1028.
    The poverty of stimulus argument is one of the most controversial arguments in the study of language acquisition. Here we follow previous approaches challenging the assumption of impoverished primary linguistic data, focusing on the specific problem of auxiliary (AUX) fronting in complex polar interrogatives. We develop a series of corpus analyses of child‐directed speech showing that there is indirect statistical information useful for correct auxiliary fronting in polar interrogatives and that such information is sufficient for distinguishing between (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  31.  35
    Partial advance information and stimulus dimensionality.Barry H. Kantowitz & Mark S. Sanders - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (3):412.
  32.  14
    Stimulus valence moderates self-learning.Parnian Jalalian, Saga Svensson, Marius Golubickis, Yadvi Sharma & C. Neil Macrae - 2024 - Cognition and Emotion 38 (6):884-897.
    Self-relevance has been demonstrated to impair instrumental learning. Compared to unfamiliar symbols associated with a friend, analogous stimuli linked with the self are learned more slowly. What is not yet understood, however, is whether this effect extends beyond arbitrary stimuli to material with intrinsically meaningful properties. Take, for example, stimulus valence an established moderator of self-bias. Does the desirability of to-be-learned material influence self-learning? Here, in conjunction with computational modelling (i.e. Reinforcement Learning Drift Diffusion Model analysis), a probabilistic selection (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  33.  1
    Does Stimulus Category Coherence Influence Visual Working Memory? A Rational Analysis.Ruoyang Hu & Robert A. Jacobs - 2024 - Cognitive Science 48 (9):e13498.
    Visual working memory (VWM) refers to the temporary storage and manipulation of visual information. Although visually different, objects we view and remember can share the same higher-level category information, such as an apple, orange, and banana all being classified as fruit. We study the influence of category information on VWM, focusing on the question of whether stimulus category coherence (i.e., whether all to-be-remembered items belong to the same semantic category) influences VWM performance. This question is addressed (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  85
    Behaviorism, finite automata, and stimulus response theory.Raymond J. Nelson - 1975 - Theory and Decision 6 (August):249-67.
    In this paper it is argued that certain stimulus-response learning models which are adequate to represent finite automata (acceptors) are not adequate to represent noninitial state input-output automata (transducers). This circumstance suggests the question whether or not the behavior of animals if satisfactorily modelled by automata is predictive. It is argued in partial answer that there are automata which can be explained in the sense that their transition and output functions can be described (roughly, Hempel-type covering law explanation) while (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  35.  28
    An information analysis of verbal and motor responses in a forced-paced serial task.Earl A. Alluisi, Paul F. Muller Jr & Paul M. Fitts - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (3):153.
  36.  22
    On the stimulus and response in pursuit tracking.E. C. Poulton - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (3):189.
  37.  19
    Emotion And Attention Interactively Regulate The Flow Of Information In V1 As Early As 75 ms After Stimulus Onset.Rossi Valentina & Pourtois Gilles - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  38.  20
    Strength of auditory stimulus-response compatability as a function of task complexity.James Callan, Diane Klisz & Oscar A. Parsons - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (6):1039.
  39.  69
    Context-specific prime-congruency effects: On the role of conscious stimulus representations for cognitive control.Alexander Heinemann, Wilfried Kunde & Andrea Kiesel - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (4):966-976.
    Recent research suggests that processing of irrelevant information can be modulated in a rapid online fashion by contextual information in the task environment depending on the usefulness of that information in different contexts. Congruency effects evoked by irrelevant stimulus attributes are smaller in contexts with high proportions of incongruent trials and larger in contexts with high proportions of congruent trials . The present study investigates these context-adaptation effects in a masked-priming paradigm. Context-specific adaptation effects transfer to (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  40.  32
    Only stimulus energy affects the detectability of visual forms and objects.Muriel Boucart & Claude Bonnet - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (5):415-417.
    A detection task was performed using different pictographic representations of objects in order to test the hypothesis that high-level information (familiarity) may influence detection thresholds. The stimuli were five versions of forms: outline drawings of objects, silhouettes, and three fragmented versions of forms derived from the outlines. The stimuli varied on two parameters: their nameability (easily nameable, hardly nameable, and not nameable) as assessed by a naming task, and their energy content as assessed by a two-dimensional fast-Fourier transform. The (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41. Visualization as a stimulus domain for vision science.Ronald A. Rensink - 2021 - Journal of Vision 21 (3):1–18.
    Traditionally, vision science and information/data visualization have interacted by using knowledge of human vision to help design effective displays. It is argued here, however, that this interaction can also go in the opposite direction: the investigation of successful visualizations can lead to the discovery of interesting new issues and phenomena in visual perception. Various studies are reviewed showing how this has been done for two areas of visualization, namely, graphical representations and interaction, which lend themselves to work on visual (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  42.  19
    Information transmission in action video gaming experts: Inferences from the lateralized readiness potential.Jiaxin Xie, Ruifang Cui, Weiyi Ma, Jingqing Lu, Lin Wang, Shaofei Ying, Dezhong Yao, Diankun Gong, Guojian Yan & Tiejun Liu - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    Research showed that action real-time strategy gaming experience is related to cognitive and neural plasticity, including visual selective attention and working memory, executive control, and information processing. This study explored the relationship between ARSG experience and information transmission in the auditory channel. Using an auditory, two-choice, go/no-go task and lateralized readiness potential as the index to partial information transmission, this study examined information transmission patterns in ARSG experts and amateurs. Results showed that experts had a higher (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  32
    The Role of Stimulus‐Specific Perceptual Fluency in Statistical Learning.Andrew Perfors & Evan Kidd - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (2):e13100.
    Humans have the ability to learn surprisingly complicated statistical information in a variety of modalities and situations, often based on relatively little input. These statistical learning (SL) skills appear to underlie many kinds of learning, but despite their ubiquity, we still do not fully understand precisely what SL is and what individual differences on SL tasks reflect. Here, we present experimental work suggesting that at least some individual differences arise from stimulus-specific variation in perceptual fluency: the ability to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  12
    Variability of Practice, Information Processing, and Decision Making—How Much Do We Know?Stanisław H. Czyż - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Decision-making is a complex action requiring efficient information processing. Specifically, in movement in which performance efficiency depends on reaction time, e.g., open-loop controlled movements, these processes may play a crucial role. Information processing includes three distinct stages, stimulus identification, response selection, and response programming. Mainly, response selection may play a substantial contribution to the reaction time and appropriate decision making. The duration of this stage depends on the number of possible choices an individual has to “screen” to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  45.  34
    The effects of post-stimulus elaboration, background valence, and item salience on the emotion-induced memory trade-off.Shu An, Weibin Mao, Sida Shang & Lili Kang - 2020 - Cognition and Emotion 34 (8):1676-1689.
    The effect of emotion on memory often leads to the trade-off: enhanced memory for emotional items comes at the cost of memory for background information. Although this effect is usually attributed...
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  24
    Reactions toward the apparent source of an auditory stimulus.J. Richard Simon, John L. Craft & A. M. Small - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 89 (1):203.
  47.  58
    Is the sunny side up and the dark side down? Effects of stimulus type and valence on a spatial detection task.Maria Amorim & Ana P. Pinheiro - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (2):346-360.
    ABSTRACTIn verbal communication, affective information is commonly conveyed to others through spatial terms. This study used a target location discrimination task with neutral, positive and negative stimuli to test the automaticity of the emotion-space association, both in the vertical and horizontal spatial axes. The effects of stimulus type on emotion-space representations were also probed. A congruency effect was observed in the vertical axis: detection of upper targets preceded by positive stimuli was faster. This effect occurred for all (...) types, indicating that the emotion-space association is not dependent on sensory modality and on the verbal content of affective stimuli. (shrink)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  48.  52
    TMS effects on subjective and objective measures of vision: Stimulation intensity and pre- versus post-stimulus masking.Tom A. de Graaf, Sonja Cornelsen, Christianne Jacobs & Alexander T. Sack - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1244-1255.
    Transcranial magnetic stimulation can be used to mask visual stimuli, disrupting visual task performance or preventing visual awareness. While TMS masking studies generally fix stimulation intensity, we hypothesized that varying the intensity of TMS pulses in a masking paradigm might inform several ongoing debates concerning TMS disruption of vision as measured subjectively versus objectively, and pre-stimulus versus post-stimulus TMS masking. We here show that both pre-stimulus TMS pulses and post-stimulus TMS pulses could strongly mask visual stimuli. (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  49.  41
    Effects of Information Overload, Communication Overload, and Inequality on Digital Distrust: A Cyber-Violence Behavior Mechanism.Mingyue Fan, Yuchen Huang, Sikandar Ali Qalati, Syed Mir Muhammad Shah, Dragana Ostic & Zhengjia Pu - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    In recent years, there has been an escalation in cases of cyber violence, which has had a chilling effect on users' behavior toward social media sites. This article explores the causes behind cyber violence and provides empirical data for developing means for effective prevention. Using elements of the stimulus–organism–response theory, we constructed a model of cyber-violence behavior. A closed-ended questionnaire was administered to collect data through an online survey, which results in 531 valid responses. A proposed model was tested (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  50.  11
    The Neural Representation of a Repeated Standard Stimulus in Dyslexia.Sara D. Beach, Ola Ozernov-Palchik, Sidney C. May, Tracy M. Centanni, Tyler K. Perrachione, Dimitrios Pantazis & John D. E. Gabrieli - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    The neural representation of a repeated stimulus is the standard against which a deviant stimulus is measured in the brain, giving rise to the well-known mismatch response. It has been suggested that individuals with dyslexia have poor implicit memory for recently repeated stimuli, such as the train of standards in an oddball paradigm. Here, we examined how the neural representation of a standard emerges over repetitions, asking whether there is less sensitivity to repetition and/or less accrual of “standardness” (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 974