Results for 'learning response compounds'

980 found
Order:
  1.  21
    Learning response compounds having two critical components.Alvin J. North & James Harrington Jr - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 47 (3):173.
  2.  23
    Compound trial-and-error learning as a function of response availability (nr). [REVIEW]Clyde E. Noble - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (2):93.
  3.  22
    Compound and simple responses in paired-associate learning.Joseph L. Young & Robert L. Schiffer - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 91 (2):206.
  4.  76
    Verbal and Behavioral Learning in a Probability Compounding Task.Daniel John Zizzo - 2003 - Theory and Decision 54 (4):287-314.
    The conjunction fallacy occurs whenever probability compounds are thought of as more likely than its component probabilities alone. In the experiment we present, subjects chose between simple and compound lotteries after some practice. Depending on the condition, they were given more or less information about the nature of probability compounds. The conjunction fallacy was surprisingly robust. There was, however, a puzzling dissociation between verbal and behavioral learning: verbal responses were sensitive, but actual choices entirely insensitive, to the (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  5. Teaching & learning guide for: Musical works: Ontology and meta-ontology.Julian Dodd - 2009 - Philosophy Compass 4 (6):1044-1048.
    A work of music is repeatable in the following sense: it can be multiply performed or played in different places at the same time, and each such datable, locatable performance or playing is an occurrence of it: an item in which the work itself is somehow present, and which thereby makes the work manifest to an audience. As I see it, the central challenge in the ontology of musical works is to come up with an ontological proposal (i.e. an account (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  33
    ""Building esprit de corps: learning to better navigate between" my" patient and" our" patient.Evan G. DeRenzo & Jack Schwartz - 2010 - Journal of Clinical Ethics 21 (3):232-237.
    Excellence in the care of hospital patients, particularly those in an intensive care unit, reflects esprit de corps among the care team. Esprit de corps depends on a delicate balance; each clinician must preserve a sense of personal responsibility for “my” patient and yet participate in the collaborative work essential to the care of “our” patient. A harmful imbalance occurs when a physician demands total control of the decision-making process, especially concerning end-of-life treatment options. Although emotional factors may push a (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  32
    Probability learning: Response proportions and verbal estimates.Lee Roy Beach, Richard M. Rose, Yutaka Sayeki, James A. Wise & William B. Carter - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 86 (2):165.
  8.  27
    Children’s paired-associate learning: Response and associative learning as a function of similarity.Robert L. Solso, John H. Mueller, Rosario C. Pesce & George Weiss - 1974 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 3 (5):327-329.
  9.  44
    Christian Ethics at the Boundary: Feminism and Theologies of Public Life by Karen V. Guth.Julie Hanlon Rubio - 2018 - Journal of the Society of Christian Ethics 38 (2):196-197.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Reviewed by:Christian Ethics at the Boundary: Feminism and Theologies of Public Life by Karen V. GuthJulie Hanlon RubioChristian Ethics at the Boundary: Feminism and Theologies of Public Life Karen V. Guth MINNEAPOLIS: FORTRESS PRESS, 2015. 231 pp. $39.00In her promising first book, Karen Guth does "ethics at the boundary," reading the central figures of Martin Luther King Jr., John Howard Yoder, and Reinhold Niebuhr with an uncommon generosity that (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  9
    Dewey, Gilligan, and Gosselin on Learning Responsibility.Ann Diller - 2003 - Philosophy of Education 59:316-318.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11. Learning Organization for Corporate Social Responsibility Implementation; Unravelling the intricate relationships between Organizational and Operational LO Characteristics.E. Osagie, R. Wesselink, Vincent Blok & M. Mulder - 2020 - Organization and Environment 1 (1).
    Because corporate social responsibility (CSR) is potentially beneficial for companies, it is important to understand the factors that improve a company’s CSR practice. Scholars hypothesize that facilitating learning organization characteristics, which are divided in characteristics at the organizational and the operational level, may improve CSR implementation. These characteristics stimulate companies and their members to be critical, learn from the past, and embrace change, but there is limited empirical evidence of this approach. This study addresses this gap by surveying 280 (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  12.  37
    Are ‘Dirty Hands’ Possible?Stephen de Wijze - 2024 - The Journal of Ethics 28 (1):187-214.
    This paper argues that ‘dirty hands’ (DH) scenarios, where an agent is forced to do wrong in order to do right, are conceptually coherent. The charge of incoherence is a widespread and common criticism made by deontologists and consequentialists alike. They argue that DH theorists erroneously assume the existence of real moral dilemmas and then compound this error by claiming that it is possible to engage in justified moral wrongdoing. However, such critics argue that there are only _prima facie_ moral (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  13.  14
    Conditioned inhibition, inhibitory learning, response inhibition, and inhibitory control: Outlining a conceptual clarification.Rodrigo Sosa - 2024 - Psychological Review 131 (1):138-173.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  44
    Taste learning in rodents: Compounds and individual taste cues recognition.Milagros Gallo - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (1):80-81.
    Contrary to the outstanding simplistic view of the taste system, learning studies show a more complex picture. Behavioral data using conditioned taste preference and aversion tasks support the idea that mixtures involve complex interactions between individual taste cues. Evidence from taste conditioned blocking, taste perceptual learning, and taste learned preferences is considered to support such a view. Greater support for research in taste learning and memory is required for a complete understanding of taste recognition.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  55
    Demarcating public from private values in evolutionary discourse.Evelyn Fox Keller - 1988 - Journal of the History of Biology 21 (2):195-211.
    What I suggest we can see in this brief overview of the literature is an extensive interpenetration on both sides of these debates between scientific, political, and social values. Important shifts in political and social values were of course occurring over the same period, some of them in parallel with, and perhaps even contributing to, these transitions I have been speaking of in evolutionary discourse. The developments that I think of as at least suggestive of possible parallels include the progressive (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  16. Learning robots and human responsibility.Dante Marino & Guglielmo Tamburrini - 2006 - International Review of Information Ethics 6:46-51.
    Epistemic limitations concerning prediction and explanation of the behaviour of robots that learn from experience are selectively examined by reference to machine learning methods and computational theories of supervised inductive learning. Moral responsibility and liability ascription problems concerning damages caused by learning robot actions are discussed in the light of these epistemic limitations. In shaping responsibility ascription policies one has to take into account the fact that robots and softbots - by combining learning with autonomy, pro-activity, (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  17.  47
    Response pretraining and subsequent paired-associate learning.Eli Saltz & Mark Felton - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (2):258.
  18.  37
    Flaubert and Sartre on Madness in King Lear.Hazel E. Barnes - 1986 - Philosophy and Literature 10 (2):211-221.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Hazel E. Barnes FLAUBERT AND SARTRE ON MADNESS IN KING LEAR T'oward the end of the second volume of The Family Idiot (L'Idiot de la famille), in a section called "Exercises and Reading," Sartre discusses Flaubert's reading of Shakespeare.1 In the context Sartre describes how Flaubert spent his time during one of the rare periods when he was not even attempting to write anything; more than two years elapsed (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  20
    Muscle responses and their relation to rote learning.R. N. Berry & R. C. Davis - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (2):188.
  20.  20
    Right-response preference in probability learning and reversal.Marilyn E. Miller - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (5):776.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  42
    Probability learning: Left-right variables and response latency.Irma R. Gerjuoy, Herbert Gerjuoy & Richard Mathias - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (4):344.
  22.  30
    Response interference in paired-associate learning.Leonard M. Horowitz & Suzanne R. Larsen - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (3):225.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23. Perceptual learning and reasons‐responsiveness.Zoe Jenkin - 2022 - Noûs 57 (2):481-508.
    Perceptual experiences are not immediately responsive to reasons. You see a stick submerged in a glass of water as bent no matter how much you know about light refraction. Due to this isolation from reasons, perception is traditionally considered outside the scope of epistemic evaluability as justified or unjustified. Is perception really as independent from reasons as visual illusions make it out to be? I argue no, drawing on psychological evidence from perceptual learning. The flexibility of perceptual learning (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  24.  46
    Explaining compound generalization in associative and causal learning through rational principles of dimensional generalization.Fabian A. Soto, Samuel J. Gershman & Yael Niv - 2014 - Psychological Review 121 (3):526-558.
  25.  41
    Service-Learning and the Socially Responsible Ethics Class.Patrick Fitzgerald - 1997 - Teaching Philosophy 20 (3):251-267.
    Despite the great promise that service learning has shown and the attention paid to it by educators, it is not commonly taken up in the courses or discussions of ethicists. But service learning should concern ethicists (especially applied ethicists) if it should concern anyone: ethicists frequently devote their courses and studies to questions of social responsibility and service learning provides a unique opportunity for students and teachers to fulfill that responsibility. After rehearsing several arguments for the basis (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  26.  24
    Response probability in a two-choice learning situation with varying probability of reinforcement.Robert H. Hickson - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (2):138.
  27. Authors’ Response: Seeking “Power” in Powerful Ideas, Systems Thinking and Affective Aspects of Learning.K. Makri, M. Daskolia & C. Kynigos - 2015 - Constructivist Foundations 10 (3):401-404.
    Upshot: The commentaries raise a plethora of issues, extending the article’s problematic in insightful ways. In this response, we chose to focus on two interesting views on the “powerful idea” in the constructionist sense, on systems versus causal-rule thinking and on the affective aspect of collaborative learning.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  29
    Learning computer ethics and social responsibility with tabletop role-playing games.Katerina Zdravkova - 2014 - Journal of Information, Communication and Ethics in Society 12 (1):60-75.
    Purpose – Tabletop online role-playing games enable active learning appropriate for different ages and learner capabilities. They have also been implemented in computer and engineering ethics courses. The paper aims to discuss these issues. Design/methodology/approach – This paper presents the experience of implementing role-playing in several courses embedded in Web 2.0 environment, with an intention to confront complex and sometimes mutually conflicting concepts, and integrate them into a whole. Findings – Typical examples introducing two basic scenarios representing individual and (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  26
    Compound-stimulus hypothesis in serial learning.Robert K. Young & James Clark - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (3):301.
  30.  57
    Learning health systems, clinical equipoise and the ethics of response adaptive randomisation.Alex John London - 2018 - Journal of Medical Ethics 44 (6):409-415.
    To give substance to the rhetoric of ‘learning health systems’, a variety of novel trial designs are being explored to more seamlessly integrate research with medical practice, reduce study duration and reduce the number of participants allocated to ineffective interventions. Many of these designs rely on response adaptive randomisation. However, critics charge that RAR is unethical on the grounds that it violates the principle of equipoise. In this paper, I reconstruct critiques of RAR as holding that it is (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  31.  30
    Compound stimuli in verbal learning: Cognitive and sensory differentiation versus stimulus selection.Eli Saltz - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (1):1.
  32.  94
    Response to Eva Alerby and Cecilia Ferm, "Learning Music: Embodied Experience in the Life-World".C. Victor Fung - 2005 - Philosophy of Music Education Review 13 (2):206-207.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Response to Eva Alerby and Cecilia Ferm, “Learning Music: Embodied Experience in the Life-World”C. Victor FungThe authors' choice of using phenomenology as a foundation of their inquiry is appropriate and appealing. They have, to a great extent, achieved their goal to explain music learning from a life-world approach. Descriptions of absolute musicality and relativistic musicality in the opening paragraphs remind me of the good old "nature (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  20
    Backward learning when the same items serve as stimuli and responses.Robert K. Young & Paul C. Jennings - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (1):64.
  34.  29
    Responsible leadership: learning from Indian case studies.Venugopal Pingali - 2016 - Asian Journal of Business Ethics 5 (1-2):139-147.
    Focus on shareholders’ interests at the cost of other stakeholders’ interests and the ecosystem has brought into prominence the need for responsible leadership. This study builds a five-level framework for responsible leadership drawing on leadership styles from case studies presented at a National Convention on Responsible Leadership. For each of the five levels, the study further identifies the necessary condition and the sufficient conditions of the leader to develop responsible organization. This comprehensive framework combines levels identified by two earlier studies. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  14
    Motor Learning in Response to Different Experimental Pain Models Among Healthy Individuals: A Systematic Review.Mohammad Izadi, Sae Franklin, Marianna Bellafiore & David W. Franklin - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    Learning new movement patterns is a normal part of daily life, but of critical importance in both sport and rehabilitation. A major question is how different sensory signals are integrated together to give rise to motor adaptation and learning. More specifically, there is growing evidence that pain can give rise to alterations in the learning process. Despite a number of studies investigating the role of pain on the learning process, there is still no systematic review to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36. Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory.Pascale Waelti, Anthony Dickinson & Wolfram Schultz - 2001 - Nature 412 (6842):43–8.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  37.  18
    Learning and frustration of responses based on positively and negatively correlated reward in children.Langdon E. Longstreth & Dunham H. Gilbert - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 79 (3p1):406.
  38.  39
    Putting responsible research and innovation into practice: a case study for biotechnology research, exploring impacts and RRI learning outcomes of public engagement for science students.Janice Limson - 2018 - Synthese 198 (Suppl 19):4685-4710.
    The responsible research and innovation framework seeks to bring science closer to society, with scientific research conducted not just for the benefit of society, but with role players in society engaging with scientists on research and innovation at every stage. A central focus of the RRI framework is the approach taken to embed these concepts in the higher education training of science students. In this study the direct engagement between science students and the public is explored as an opportunity for (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  39.  31
    Response latencies produced by massed and spaced learning of a paired-associates list.John Brown & M. Huda - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (5):360.
  40. Response to Eva Alerby and Cecilia Ferm, "Learning Music: Embodied Experience in the Life-World".Christine A. Brown - 2005 - Philosophy of Music Education Review 13 (2):208-210.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Response to Eva Alerby and Cecilia Ferm, “Learning Music: Embodied Experience in the Life-World”Christine A. BrownI was recently asked to settle a friendly debate between two college graduates. The first, my daughter's boyfriend, argued that someone with talent and motivation could become as creative a composer without formal musical training as with it. The other, my daughter, vigorously countered that while someone might compose well on one's (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  20
    Learning of responses to stimuli classes and to specific stimuli.Burton H. Cohen & Peter A. Hut - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (3):274.
  42.  15
    Compound conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response: Test trial manipulations.E. James Kehoe & Bernard G. Schreurs - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (1):79-81.
  43.  29
    Response properties of the position indicant in serial learning.John H. Mueller - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 84 (1):35.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44. Authors' Response: Learning, Anticipation and the Brain.E. B. Roesch, M. Spencer, S. J. Nasuto, T. Tanay & J. M. Bishop - 2013 - Constructivist Foundations 9 (1):42-45.
    Upshot: Albeit mostly supportive of our work, the commentaries we received highlighted a few points that deserve additional explanation, with regard to the notion of learning in our model, the relationship between our model and the brain, as well as the notion of anticipation. This open discussion emphasizes the need for toy computer models, to fuel theoretical discussion and prevent business-as-usual from getting in the way of new ideas.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  21
    Learning and retention of paired adjectives as a function of percentage occurrence of response members.Rudolph W. Schulz & Willard N. Runquist - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (6):409.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  42
    Learning of a hurdle-jump response to escape cues paired with reduced reward or frustrative nonreward.Helen B. Daly - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 79 (1p1):146.
  47. Response to M. Vicentini's “comment on the article 'studying conceptual change in learning physics'”.Dewy I. Dykstra, C. Franklin Boyle & Ira A. Monarch - 1993 - Science Education 77 (6):717-723.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  22
    Compound stimuli in paired-associate learning.Leonard M. Horowitz, Louis G. Kippman & George W. McConkie - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (2):132.
  49.  51
    Response to Megan Laverty’s Review of Beyond Learning.Gert Biesta - 2009 - Studies in Philosophy and Education 28 (6):577-579.
  50.  31
    Response-To-Intervention in Finland and the United States: Mathematics Learning Support as an Example.Piia M. Björn, Mikko Aro, Tuire Koponen, Lynn S. Fuchs & Douglas Fuchs - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 980