Results for ' stimulus response similarity'

988 found
Order:
  1.  20
    Formulation of a generalization surface for the simultaneous variation of stimulus and response similarity.Michael Shea - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 80 (2p1):353.
  2.  34
    Transfer as a function of stimulus, response, and simultaneous stimulus and response similarity.Barbara S. Uehling & Benton J. Underwood - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 95 (2):375.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  26
    Two roads leading to the same evaluative conditioning effect? Stimulus-response binding versus operant conditioning.Tarini Singh, Christian Frings & Eva Walther - 2024 - Cognition and Emotion 38 (5):825-833.
    Evaluative Conditioning (EC) refers to changes in our liking or disliking of a stimulus due to its pairing with other positive or negative stimuli. In addition to stimulus-based mechanisms, recent research has shown that action-based mechanisms can also lead to EC effects. Research, based on action control theories, has shown that pairing a positive or negative action with a neutral stimulus results in EC effects (Stimulus-Response binding). Similarly, research studies using Operant Conditioning (OC) approaches have (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  39
    Stimulus generalization of the conditioned eyelid response to structurally similar nonsense syllables.David W. Abbott & Louis E. Price - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (4):368.
  5.  26
    The Incomplete Tyranny of Dynamic Stimuli: Gaze Similarity Predicts Response Similarity in Screen‐Captured Instructional Videos.Daniel T. Levin, Jorge A. Salas, Anna M. Wright, Adrianne E. Seiffert, Kelly E. Carter & Joshua W. Little - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (6):e12984.
    Although eye tracking has been used extensively to assess cognitions for static stimuli, recent research suggests that the link between gaze and cognition may be more tenuous for dynamic stimuli such as videos. Part of the difficulty in convincingly linking gaze with cognition is that in dynamic stimuli, gaze position is strongly influenced by exogenous cues such as object motion. However, tests of the gaze‐cognition link in dynamic stimuli have been done on only a limited range of stimuli often characterized (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6.  28
    Effects of task difficulty, stimulus similarity, and type of response on stimulus predifferentiation.Bennet B. Murdock Jr - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (2):167.
  7.  34
    Choice response times as functions of intralist similarity, stimulus type, and number of equally probable alternatives.Barry Gholson & Raymond H. Hohle - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (3):581.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  30
    Transfer in verbal materials with dissimilar stimuli and response similarity varied.Robert K. Young & Benton J. Underwood - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 47 (3):153.
  9.  20
    The relationship of stimulus similarity and number of responses.Jack Richardson - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 56 (6):478.
  10.  24
    Mediating verbal responses and stimulus similarity as factors in conceptual naming by school age children.Harvey M. Lacey - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (2):113.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  37
    The effect of stimulus similarity on the acquisition and extinction of a conditioned response.Darwin P. Hunt - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (3):278.
  12.  34
    Paired-associate learning as a function of similarity: Common stimulus and response items within the list.Takao Umemoto & Ernest R. Hilgard - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (2):97.
  13.  22
    Generalization of reinforcement among similar responses made in altered stimulus situations.Melvin H. Marx & Benjamin B. Bernstein - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 50 (6):355.
  14.  32
    Differential relation of latency and response vigor to stimulus similarity in brightness discrimination.Alfred Castaneda & Leonard Worell - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (4):309.
  15.  28
    Differential eyelid conditioning as a function of stimulus similarity and strength of response to the CS.Malcolm D. Gynther - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (6):408.
  16.  27
    Studies of distributed practice: IX. Learning and retention of paired adjectives as a function of intralist similarity.Benton J. Underwood - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 45 (3):143.
  17.  14
    Visual Modulation of Human Responses to Support Surface Translation.Mustafa Emre Akçay, Vittorio Lippi & Thomas Mergner - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Vision is known to improve human postural responses to external perturbations. This study investigates the role of vision for the responses to continuous pseudorandom support surface translations in the body sagittal plane in three visual conditions: with the eyes closed, in stroboscopic illumination and with eyes open in continuous illumination with the room as static visual scene. In the frequency spectrum of the translation stimulus we distinguished on the basis of the response patterns between a low-frequency, mid-frequency, and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  18.  18
    Mechanisms of False Alarm in Response to Fear Stimulus: An Event-Related Potential Study.Xiai Wang, Jicheng Sun, Jinghua Yang, Shan Cheng, Cui Liu, Wendong Hu & Jin Ma - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Background and ObjectiveThere is a paucity of research that has explored “False Alarm” mechanisms. In order to remedy this deficiency in knowledge, the present study used event-related potential technology to reveal the mechanisms underlying False Alarm in response to fear stimuli.MethodsThis study selected snakes as experimental materials and the “oddball paradigm” was used to simulate the conditions of False Alarm. The mechanism underlying False Alarm was revealed by comparing cognitive processing similarities and differences between real snakes and toy snakes.ResultsEvent-related (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  26
    Paired-associate learning when the same items occur as stimuli and responses.Robert K. Young - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (4):315.
  20.  32
    Stimulus recall following paired-associate learning.Samuel M. Feldman & Benton J. Underwood - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (1):11.
  21.  29
    Generalization to similar and opposite words.Richard K. Olson - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (3):328.
  22.  6
    Stimulus processing bias in anxiety-related fear generalisation: drift-diffusion modelling and subgroups differences.Donghuan Zhang, Min Fan, Biyao Zhang, Yixuan Feng, Gao Yu, Wei Chen, Feng Biao & Xifu Zheng - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    In fear differential conditioning, stimuli that resemble the conditioned stimulus (CS+) are more likely to trigger fear responses. Excessive fear responses on stimuli not like CS + are often associated with anxiety. However, the threat judgments process and how this process manifests itself differently in subgroups with different generalisation rule applications, is unclear. This study examines whether anxiety biases the threat decision process in fear generalisation paradigm and whether subgroups characterised by different generalisation gradients was interpreted differently by drift-diffusion (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  33
    Suppressing the Chills: Effects of Musical Manipulation on the Chills Response.Scott Bannister & Tuomas Eerola - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:412115.
    Research on musical chills has linked the response to multiple musical features; however, there exists no study that has attempted to manipulate musical stimuli to enable causal inferences, meaning current understanding is based mainly on correlational evidence. In the current study, participants who regularly experience chills ( N = 24) listened to an original and manipulated version of three pieces reported to elicit chills in a previous survey. Predefined chills sections were removed to create manipulated conditions. The effects of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  24.  71
    Preliminary evidence for selective cortical responses to music in one‐month‐old infants.Heather Kosakowski, Samuel Norman-Haignere, Anna Mynick, Atsushi Takahashi, Rebecca Saxe & Nancy Kanwisher - 2023 - Developmental Science 26 (5):e13387.
    Prior studies have observed selective neural responses in the adult human auditory cortex to music and speech that cannot be explained by the differing lower-level acoustic properties of these stimuli. Does infant cortex exhibit similarly selective responses to music and speech shortly after birth? To answer this question, we attempted to collect functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data from 45 sleeping infants (2.0- to 11.9-weeks-old) while they listened to monophonic instrumental lullabies and infant-directed speech produced by a mother. To match (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  83
    Merging the Psychophysical Function With Response Times for Auditory Detection of One vs. Two Tones.Jennifer J. Lentz & James T. Townsend - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    The purpose of this study is to take preliminary steps to unify psychoacoustic techniques with reaction-time methodologies to address the perceptual mechanisms responsible for the detection of one vs. multiple sounds. We measured auditory redundancy gains for auditory detection of pure tones widely spaced in frequency using the tools of Systems Factorial Technology to evince the system architecture and workload capacity in two different scenarios. We adopted an experimental design in which the presence or absence of a target at each (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  22
    Influence of the Location of a Decision Cue on the Dynamics of Pupillary Light Response.Pragya Pandey & Supriya Ray - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    The pupils of the eyes reflexively constrict in light and dilate in dark to optimize retinal illumination. Non-visual cognitive factors, like attention, arousal, decision-making, etc., also influence pupillary light response. During passive viewing, the eccentricity of a stimulus modulates the pupillary aperture size driven by spatially weighted corneal flux density, which is the product of luminance and the area of the stimulus. Whether the scope of attention also influences PLR remains unclear. In this study, we contrasted the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  28
    Symbolic Processes and Stimulus Equivalence.Ullin T. Place - 1995 - Behavior and Philosophy 23 (3-1):13 - 30.
    A symbol is defined as a species of sign. The concept of a sign coincides with Skinner's (1938) concept of a discriminative stimulus. Symbols differ from other signs in five respects: (1) They are stimuli which the organism can both respond to and produce, either as a self-directed stimulus (as in thinking) or as a stimulus for another individual with a predictably similar response from the recipient in each case. (2) they act as discriminative stimuli for (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28.  58
    Temporal delays, not underactivation of detection processes may be responsible for neglect.I.-han Chou & Peter H. Schiller - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):675-676.
    We have shown that FEF lesion-induced extinction could be compensated for by changing the relative temporal onsets of two targets presented on either side of the midline. Monkeys were trained to make saccades to either of two identical visual stimuli presented with various stimulus onset asynchronies (SOA). In intact animals the targets were chosen with equal probability when they appeared simultaneously. After unilateral FEF lesions an SOA of 67–116 msec had to be introduced, with the contralesional target appearing first, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  47
    A Quantitative Account of the Behavioral Characteristics of Habituation: The Sometimes Opponent Processes Model of Stimulus Processing.Yerco E. Uribe-Bahamonde, Sebastián A. Becerra, Fernando P. Ponce & Edgar H. Vogel - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Habituation is defined as a decline in responding to a repeated stimulus. After more than eighty years of research, there is an enduring consensus among researchers on the existence of 9-10 behavioral regularities or parameters of habituation. There is no similar agreement, however, on the best approach to explain these facts. In this paper, we demonstrate that the Sometimes Opponent Processes (SOP) model of stimulus processing accurately describes all of these regularities. This model was proposed by Allan Wagner (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  30.  13
    Do gaze and non-gaze stimuli trigger different spatial interference effects? It depends on stimulus perceivability.Zhe Chen, Rebecca H. Thomas & Makayla S. Chen - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Among the studies on the perception of gaze vs. non-gaze stimuli, some have shown that the two types of stimuli trigger different patterns of attentional effects, while others have reported no such differences. In three experiments, we investigated the role of stimulus perceivability in spatial interference effects when the targets were gaze vs. non-gaze stimuli. We used a spatial Stroop task that required participants to make a speeded response to the direction indicated by the targets located on the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  11
    The Neural Representation of a Repeated Standard Stimulus in Dyslexia.Sara D. Beach, Ola Ozernov-Palchik, Sidney C. May, Tracy M. Centanni, Tyler K. Perrachione, Dimitrios Pantazis & John D. E. Gabrieli - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    The neural representation of a repeated stimulus is the standard against which a deviant stimulus is measured in the brain, giving rise to the well-known mismatch response. It has been suggested that individuals with dyslexia have poor implicit memory for recently repeated stimuli, such as the train of standards in an oddball paradigm. Here, we examined how the neural representation of a standard emerges over repetitions, asking whether there is less sensitivity to repetition and/or less accrual of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32.  87
    Hippocampus, space, and memory.David S. Olton, James T. Becker & Gail E. Handelmann - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):313-322.
    We examine two different descriptions of the behavioral functions of the hippocampal system. One emphasizes spatially organized behaviors, especially those using cognitive maps. The other emphasizes memory, particularly working memory, a short-term memory that requires iexible stimulus-response associations and is highly susceptible to interference. The predictive value of the spatial and memory descriptions were evaluated by testing rats with damage to the hippocampal system in a series of experiments, independently manipulating the spatial and memory characteristics of a behavioral (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   292 citations  
  33.  39
    Analytical modeling of the hysteresis phenomenon in guinea pig ventricular myocytes.Paco Lorente, Carmen Delgado, Mario Delmar & Jose Jalife - 1992 - Acta Biotheoretica 40 (2):177-193.
    In the present study, we have demonstrated hysteresis phenomena in the excitability of single, enzymatically dissociated guinea pig ventricular myocytes. Membrane potentials were recorded with patch pipettes in the whole-cell current clamp configuration. Repetitive stimulation with depolarizing current pulses of constant cycle length and duration but varying strength led to predictable excitation (1:l) and non-excitation (1:0) patterns depending on current strength. In addition, transition between patterns depended on the direction of current intensity change and stable hysteresis loops were obtained in (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  80
    The roots of epistemological 'anarchy'.Nancy J. Nersessian - 1979 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 22 (1-4):423 – 440.
    The claims of the epistemological 'anarchists' have their roots in the orthodox tradition as well as in the Popperian. In particular they follow from the work of Quine. Meaning variance and incommensurability follow directly from the holistic conception of meaning in his 'network' view. Quine's efforts to evade this conclusion fail. His attempt to develop a theory-neutral notion of observation sentence is shown (1) to be inconsistent with his previous claims since it involves the tacit acceptance of the 'dogma of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  35. The Arts and the Creation of Mind: Eisner's Contributions to the Arts in Education.Arthur Efland - 2004 - Journal of Aesthetic Education 38 (4):71.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:The Journal of Aesthetic Education 38.4 (2004) 71-80 [Access article in PDF] The Arts and the Creation of Mind: Eisner's Contributions to the Arts in Education Arthur Efland Professor Emeritus, Department of Art Education The Ohio State University In the last four years at least three books in arts education have dealt with the subject of cognition in relation to the arts. I refer to Charles Dorn's Mind in (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  75
    Interoceptive awareness and unaware fear conditioning: Are subliminal conditioning effects influenced by the manipulation of visceral self-perception?An K. Raes & Rudi De Raedt - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1393-1402.
    Research has shown repeatedly that attention influences implicit learning effects. In a similar vein, interoceptive awareness might be involved in unaware fear conditioning: The fact that the CS is repeatedly presented in the context of aversive bodily experiences might facilitate the development of conditioned responding. We investigated the role of interoceptive attention in a subliminal conditioning paradigm. Conditioning was embedded in a spatial cueing task with subliminally presented cues that were followed by a masking stimulus. Response times to (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  87
    Religion as Language.Jerome I. Gellman - 1985 - Religious Studies 21 (2):159 - 168.
    When are sentences A and B the same belief? Following Quine, observation sentences A and B are the same belief when they share the same stimulus–meaning, similar patterns of assent and dissent by subjects when the sentences are queried in the presence of the same non–linguistic stimuli. As for non–observation sentences we note a suggestion of Karl Schick: apply linguistic stimuli in the form of utterances of the language, and map the connections between sentences in the language in terms (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  99
    Defending a Phenomenological–Behavioral Perspective: Culture, Behavior, and Experience.Marino Pérez-Álvarez, José M. García-Montes, Adolfo J. Cangas & Louis A. Sass - 2008 - Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology 15 (3):281-285.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Defending a Phenomenological–Behavioral Perspective: Culture, Behavior, and ExperienceMarino Pérez-Álvarez (bio), José M. García-Montes (bio), Adolfo J. Cangas (bio), and Louis A. Sass (bio)KeywordsBehavior, contextual phenomenology, culture, experienceWe should like to express our sincere thanks to all the authors for their commentaries on our articles. Given the restrictions of space (a limitation they too had to contend with), we can only respond to a few aspects of their interesting remarks. (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  50
    Treading Water in Neurath's Ship: Quine, Davidson, Rorty.Christopher Norris - 1998 - Principia: An International Journal of Epistemology 2 (2):227–280.
    This article examines what I take to be some of the wrong turns and false dilemmas that analytic philosophy has run into since Quine's well-known attack on the two 'last dogmas' of old-style Logical Empiricism. In particular it traces the consequences of Quine's argument for a thoroughly naturalized epistemology, one that would view philosophy of science as 'all the philosophy we need', and that defines 'philosophy of science' in narrowly physicalist terms. I contend that this amounts to a third residual (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  19
    Effect of stimulus-response meaningfulness on paired-associate learning and retention.V. K. Kothurkar - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (3):305.
  41.  9
    Stimulus-Response Theorie endlicher Automaten.P. Suppes - 1983 - In Wolfgang Balzer & Michael Heidelberger, Zur Logik Empirischer Theorien. De Gruyter. pp. 245-280.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42. A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent.O. H. Mowrer - 1939 - Psychological Review 46 (6):553-565.
  43.  31
    Age differences in transfer and retroaction as a function of intertask response similarity.Michael Gladis & Harry W. Braun - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (1):25.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  18
    Stimulus-response coding and amount of information as determinants of reaction time.Sidney Hellyer - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (5):521.
  45.  33
    Stimulus-response compatibility as a determinant of interference in a Stroop-like task.Elaine Fox - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (5):377-380.
  46.  38
    Stimulus-response generalization with discrete response choices.Gustav Levine - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (1):23.
  47.  23
    Stimulus-response contiguity in classical aversive conditioning.R. A. Champion - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (1):35.
  48.  33
    The stimulus-response crisis.Robyn Wilford, Juan Ardila-Cifuentes, Edward Baggs & Michael L. Anderson - 2022 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 45.
    Yarkoni correctly recognizes that one reason for psychology's generalizability crisis is the failure to account for variance within experiments. We argue that this problem, and the generalizability crisis broadly, is a necessary consequence of the stimulus-response paradigm widely used in psychology research. We point to another methodology, perturbation experiments, as a remedy that is not vulnerable to the same problems.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  87
    Stimulus, response, meaning.Jonathan Bennett - unknown
  50. Associations to stimulus-response theories of language.Thomas G. Bever - 1968 - In T. Dixon & Deryck Horton, Verbal Behavior and General Behavior Theory. Prentice-Hall. pp. 478--494.
1 — 50 / 988