Results for ' motor behaviour'

972 found
Order:
  1.  60
    Thermodynamic study of motor behaviour optimization.Patrick Cordier, Michel Mendès France, Philippe Bolon & Jean Pailhous - 1994 - Acta Biotheoretica 42 (2-3):187-201.
    Our work is aimed at studying the optimization of a complex motor behaviour from a global perspective. First, free climbing as a sport will be briefly introduced while emphasizing in particular its psychomotor aspect called route finding. The basic question raised here is how does the optimization of a sensorimotoricity-environment system take place. The material under study is the free climber's trajectory, viewed as the signature of climbing behaviour (i.e., the spatial dimension). The concepts of learning, optimization, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  2.  11
    Internal Mechanisms of Human Motor Behaviour: A System-Theoretical Perspective.Wacław Petryński, Robert Staszkiewicz & Mirosław Szyndera - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13:841343.
    The authors present the conceptual and system-theoretical model of human motor behaviour. The main assumption is that movement is the only observable manifestation of all psychical processes, thus, it is the only phenomenon enabling the creation of hypotheses concerning the psychological conditioning of human behaviour. They pointed to the fact that in the field of biology, and all the more, in psychology, mathematical descriptions are hardly eligible. In this respect, a system-theoretical approach seems to be appropriate. The (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  21
    Habituation and Dishabituation in Motor Behavior: Experiment and Neural Dynamic Model.Sophie Aerdker, Jing Feng & Gregor Schöner - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Does motor behavior early in development have the same signatures of habituation, dishabituation, and Spencer-Thompson dishabituation known from infant perception and cognition? And do these signatures explain the choice preferences in A not B motor decision tasks? We provide new empirical evidence that gives an affirmative answer to the first question together with a unified neural dynamic model that gives an affirmative answer to the second question.In the perceptual and cognitive domains, habituation is the weakening of an orientation (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  43
    Factors affecting athletes’ motor behavior after the observation of scenes of cooperation and competition in competitive sport: the effect of sport attitude.Elisa De Stefani, Doriana De Marco & Maurizio Gentilucci - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  5. Speech-controlled motor behavior and problem solutions-what is relevant.Ef Shipley - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (6):478-478.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  13
    Characterization of Sensory-Motor Behavior Under Cognitive Load Using a New Statistical Platform for Studies of Embodied Cognition.Jihye Ryu & Elizabeth B. Torres - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  7.  36
    Identifying units of motor behavior.Richard A. Schmidt - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):163-164.
  8.  31
    Grounding the meaning of non-prototypical smiles on motor behavior.Timothy A. Mann & Yoonsuck Choe - 2010 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (6):453-454.
    We address how the motor system can contribute to the component of smile perception. A smile perceiver can ground the meaning of non-prototypical smiles by interacting with the presenter to maintain the presenter's type of smile. In this case, the meaning of that smile is congruent with the motor behavior that elicits that smile (such as a funny gesture).
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  21
    Moving with control: Using control theory to understand motor behavior.Neville Hogan - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):550-551.
  10.  47
    Passing thoughts on the evolutionary stability of implicit motor behaviour: Performance retention under physiological fatigue.J. M. Poolton, R. S. W. Masters & J. P. Maxwell - 2007 - Consciousness and Cognition 16 (2):456-468.
    Heuristics of evolutionary biology dictate that phylogenetically older processes are inherently more stable and resilient to disruption than younger processes. On the grounds that non-declarative behaviour emerged long before declarative behaviour, Reber argues that implicit learning is supported by neural processes that are evolutionarily older than those supporting explicit learning. Reber suggested that implicit learning thus leads to performance that is more robust than explicit learning. Applying this evolutionary framework to motor performance, we examined whether implicit (...) learning, relative to explicit motor learning, conferred motor output that was resilient to physiological fatigue and durable over time. In Part One of the study a fatigued state was induced by a double Wingate Anaerobic test protocol. Fatigue had no affect on performance of participants in the implicit condition; whereas, performance of participants in the explicit condition deteriorated significantly. In Part Two of the study a convenience sample of participants was recalled following a one-year hiatus. In both the implicit and the explicit condition retention of performance was seen and, contrary to the findings in Part One, so was resilience to fatigue. The resilient performance in the explicit condition after one year may have resulted from forgetting or from consolidation of declarative knowledge as implicit memories. In either case, implicit processes were left to more effectively support motor performance. (shrink)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  11.  49
    Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex modulates supplementary motor area in coordinated unimanual motor behavior.Avisa Asemi, Karthik Ramaseshan, Ashley Burgess, Vaibhav A. Diwadkar & Steven L. Bressler - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  12.  32
    Teaching interactions are based on motor behavior embodiment.Ludovic Marin - 2015 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  38
    Walking in School-Aged Children in a Dual-Task Paradigm Is Related to Age But Not to Cognition, Motor Behavior, Injuries, or Psychosocial Functioning.Priska Hagmann-von Arx, Olivia Manicolo, Sakari Lemola & Alexander Grob - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  14.  25
    Response-specific effects of pain observation on motor behavior.India Morrison, Ellen Poliakoff, Lucy Gordon & Paul Downing - 2007 - Cognition 104 (2):407-416.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  15.  29
    Functional architectures and structured flows on manifolds: A dynamical framework for motor behavior.Raoul Huys, Dionysios Perdikis & Viktor K. Jirsa - 2014 - Psychological Review 121 (3):302-336.
  16.  19
    (1 other version)Visual target distance, but not visual cursor path length produces shifts in motor behavior: a multisensory integration perspective.Loes C. J. Van Dam - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  30
    Polanyi's “From-To” Knowing and His Contribution to the Phenomenology of Skilled Motor Behavior.Peter Hopsicker - 2009 - Journal of the Philosophy of Sport 36 (1):76-87.
  18.  17
    Negative Body Image Is Not Related to Spontaneous Body-Scaled Motoric Behavior in Undergraduate Women.Klaske A. Glashouwer, Charlotte Meulman & Peter J. de Jong - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19. Differential involvement of the prefrontal, premotor, and primary motor cortices in rule-based motor behavior.Eiji Hoshi - 2008 - In Silvia A. Bunge & Jonathan D. Wallis (eds.), Neuroscience of rule-guided behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.
  20.  21
    The application of operational analysis to human motor behavior.Douglas G. Ellson - 1949 - Psychological Review 56 (1):9-17.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  23
    Editorial: Reaching to Grasp Cognition: Analyzing Motor Behavior to Investigate Social Interactions.Claudia Gianelli & Maurizio Gentilucci - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  22
    Movement: From mechanisms to behaviour. Coordination of motor behaviour. Edited by B. M. H. Bush and F. Clarac. SEB Seminar Series, no. 24. Cambridge University Press, pp. 324. £20, $34.50. [REVIEW]J. F. Iles - 1986 - Bioessays 4 (4):187-187.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  60
    Does visual-field specialization really have implications for coordinated visual-motor behavior?Richard A. Abrams - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):542-543.
  24.  11
    Editorial: Current Issues in Perceptual Training: Facing the Requirement to Couple Perception, Cognition, and Action in Complex Motor Behavior.André Klostermann & David L. Mann - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25. Motor compatibility: The bidirectional link between behavior and evaluation.Roland Neumann, Jens Förster & Fritz Strack - 2003 - In Jochen Musch & Karl C. Klauer (eds.), The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion. Lawerence Erlbaum. pp. 371--391.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  26.  30
    Motor conflict behavior as a function of motivation and amount of training.Burton G. Andreas - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (2):173.
  27.  23
    Exploration of sensory-motor tradeoff behavior in Parkinson’s disease.Sonal Sengupta, W. Pieter Medendorp, Luc P. J. Selen & Peter Praamstra - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16:951313.
    While slowness of movement is an obligatory characteristic of Parkinson’s disease (PD), there are conditions in which patients move uncharacteristically fast, attributed to deficient motor inhibition. Here we investigate deficient inhibition in an optimal sensory-motor integration framework, using a game in which subjects used a paddle to catch a virtual ball. Display of the ball was extinguished as soon as the catching movement started, segregating the task into a sensing and acting phase. We analyzed the behavior of 9 (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  20
    Telling Apart Motor Noise and Exploratory Behavior, in Early Development.Teodora Gliga - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:344150.
    Infants’ minutes long babbling bouts or repetitive reaching for or mouthing of whatever they can get their hands on gives very much the impression of active exploration, a building block for early learning. But how can we tell apart active exploration from the activity of an immature motor system, attempting but failing to achieve goal directed behavior? I will focus here on evidence that infants increase motor activity and variability when faced with opportunities to gather new information (about (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  29.  47
    Voluntary behavior in cognitive and motor tasks.Heidi Kloos & Guy Van Orden - 2010 - Mind and Matter 8 (1):19-43.
    Many previous treatments of voluntary behavior have viewed intentions as causes of behavior. This has resulted in several dilemmas, including a dilemma concerning the origin of intentions. The present article circumvents traditional dilemmas by treating intentions as constraints that restrict degrees of freedom for behavior. Constraints self-organize as temporary dynamic structures that span the mind-body divide. This treatment of intentions and voluntary behavior yields a theory of intentionality that is consistent with existing findings and supported by current research.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  30.  29
    Erratum to “Passing thoughts on the evolutionary stability of implicit motor behaviour: Performance retention under physiological fatigue” [Consiousness and Cognition, 16, 456–468, 2007]. [REVIEW]J. M. Poolton, R. S. W. Masters & J. P. Maxwell - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (1):408-408.
  31.  21
    Motor speech deficits in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia.Poole Matthew, Brodtmann Amy, Pemberton Hugh, Low Essie, Darby David & Vogel Adam - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  32.  18
    The Interrelationship Between Motor Coordination and Adaptive Behavior in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder.Emily Bremer & John Cairney - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  26
    A Perceptual Motor Intervention Improves Play Behavior in Children with Moderate to Severe Cerebral Palsy.Brigette O. Ryalls, Regina Harbourne, Lisa Kelly-Vance, Jordan Wickstrom, Nick Stergiou & Anastasia Kyvelidou - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  34.  38
    On phenomenological and logical characteristics of skilled behaviour in sport: cognitive and motor intentionality.Vegard Fusche Moe - 2016 - Sport, Ethics and Philosophy 10 (3):251-268.
    In this paper, I discuss phenomenological and logical characteristics of skilled behaviour in sport. The paper comprises two parts. The first describes phenomenological characteristics of skilled behaviour through Timothy Gallwey’s two playing modes and Maurice Merleau-Ponty’s distinction between abstract and concrete movement. The second logical part introduces the concept of intentionality and the distinction Sean Kelly makes between cognitive and motor intentionality. I discuss how this distinction fits the phenomenological characteristics established in the first part of the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  35.  29
    Perceptual-Cognitive Changes During Motor Learning: The Influence of Mental and Physical Practice on Mental Representation, Gaze Behavior, and Performance of a Complex Action.Cornelia Frank, William M. Land & Thomas Schack - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  36.  36
    Multiple Sensory‐Motor Pathways Lead to Coordinated Visual Attention.Chen Yu & Linda B. Smith - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S1):5-31.
    Joint attention has been extensively studied in the developmental literature because of overwhelming evidence that the ability to socially coordinate visual attention to an object is essential to healthy developmental outcomes, including language learning. The goal of this study was to understand the complex system of sensory-motor behaviors that may underlie the establishment of joint attention between parents and toddlers. In an experimental task, parents and toddlers played together with multiple toys. We objectively measured joint attention—and the sensory-motor (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  37.  30
    Effects of Cognitive Control Exertion and Motor Coordination on Task Self-Efficacy and Muscular Endurance Performance in Children.Jeffrey D. Graham, Yao-Chuen Li, Steven R. Bray & John Cairney - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12:347028.
    Emerging research shows a strong connection between brain areas governing cognition and motor behavior. Yet, research investigating the negative aftereffects of cognitive control exertion on task performance has not considered the potential role of areas governing motor behavior. The present study investigated the effects of high cognitive control exertion on task self-efficacy and exercise performance in children. A secondary purpose was to investigate whether motor coordination influences the change in exercise performance differently following low versus high cognitive (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  15
    Individual and collective behavior of vibrating motors interacting through a resonant plate.David Mertens & Richard Weaver - 2011 - Complexity 16 (5):45-53.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  7
    New Insights Into Sedentary Behavior Highlight the Need to Revisit the Way We See Motor Symptoms in Psychosis.Vijay A. Mittal, Jessica A. Bernard, Gregory P. Strauss & Sebastian Walther - 2021 - Schizophrenia Bulletin 47 (4):877-879.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  21
    Action representations in prevention behavior: Evidence from motor execution.Solveig Tonn, Moritz Schaaf, Wilfried Kunde & Roland Pfister - 2023 - Cognition 234 (C):105370.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  67
    Implicit short-lived motor representations of space in brain damaged and healthy subjects.Yves Rossetti - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):520-558.
    This article reviews experimental evidence for a specific sensorimotor function which can be dissociated from higher level representations of space. It attempts to delineate this function on the basis of results obtained by psychophysical experiments performed with brain damaged and healthy subjects. Eye and hand movement control exhibit automatic features, such that they are incompatible with conscious control. In addition, they rely on a reference frame different from the one used by conscious perception. Neuropsychological cases provide a strong support for (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  42.  21
    New findings on the behavior of supplementary motor area neurons recorded from task-performing monkeys.Jun Tanji - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):599-600.
  43.  8
    Motor constellation theory: A model of infants’ phonological development.Axel G. Ekström - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Every normally developing human infant solves the difficult problem of mapping their native-language phonology, but the neural mechanisms underpinning this behavior remain poorly understood. Here, motor constellation theory, an integrative neurophonological model, is presented, with the goal of explicating this issue. It is assumed that infants’ motor-auditory phonological mapping takes place through infants’ orosensory “reaching” for phonological elements observed in the language-specific ambient phonology, via reference to kinesthetic feedback from motor systems, and auditory feedback from resulting speech (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  49
    Influence of Bilateral Motor Behaviors on Flexible Functioning: An Embodied Perspective.Joël Cretenet & Vincent Dru - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (6):1139-1161.
    To examine the influence of bilateral motor behaviors on flexibility performance, two studies were conducted. Previous research has shown that when performing unilateral motor behavior that activates the affective and motivational systems of approach versus avoidance (arm flexion vs. extension), it is the congruence between laterality and motor activation that determines flexibility-rigidity functioning (Cretenet & Dru, 2009). When bilateral motor behaviors were performed, a mechanism of embodiment was revealed. It showed that the flexibility scores were determined (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  56
    Stable implicit motor processes despite aerobic locomotor fatigue.R. S. W. Masters, J. M. Poolton & J. P. Maxwell - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (1):335-338.
    Implicit processes almost certainly preceded explicit processes in our evolutionary history, so they are likely to be more resistant to disruption according to the principles of evolutionary biology [Reber, A. S. . The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective. Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 93–133.]. Previous work . Knowledge, nerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343–358.]) has shown that implicitly learned motor (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  46. The Proactive Synergy Between Action Observation and Execution in the Acquisition of New Motor Skills.Maria Chiara Bazzini, Arturo Nuara, Emilia Scalona, Doriana De Marco, Giacomo Rizzolatti, Pietro Avanzini & Maddalena Fabbri-Destro - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16:793849.
    Motor learning can be defined as a process that leads to relatively permanent changes in motor behavior through repeated interactions with the environment. Different strategies can be adopted to achieve motor learning: movements can be overtly practiced leading to an amelioration of motor performance; alternatively, covert strategies (e.g., action observation) can promote neuroplastic changes in the motor system even in the absence of real movement execution. However, whether a training regularly alternating action observation and execution (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  47.  90
    Supplementary motor area structure and function: review and hypotheses.Gary Goldberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):567-588.
  48.  21
    Learning how to combine sensory-motor functions into a robust behavior.Benoit Morisset & Malik Ghallab - 2008 - Artificial Intelligence 172 (4-5):392-412.
  49.  19
    Immersive Technology for Cognitive-Motor Training in Parkinson’s Disease.Justin Lau, Claude Regis, Christina Burke, MaryJo Kaleda, Raymond McKenna & Lisa M. Muratori - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    Background: Parkinson’s disease is a neurodegenerative disease in which the progressive loss of dopaminergic neurons leads to initially sporadic and eventually widespread damage of the nervous system resulting in significant musculoskeletal and cognitive deterioration. Loss of motor function alongside increasing cognitive impairment is part of the natural disease progression. Gait is often considered an automatic activity; however, walking is the result of a delicate balance of multiple systems which maintain the body’s center of mass over an ever-changing base of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  34
    Auditory-motor synchronization with temporally fluctuating sequences is dependent on fractal structure but not musical expertise.Summer K. Rankin & Charles J. Limb - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:103164.
    Fractal structure is a ubiquitous property found in nature and biology, and has been observed in processes at different levels of organization, including rhythmic behavior and musical structure. A temporal process is characterized as fractal when serial long-term correlations and statistical self-similarity (scaling) are present. Previous studies of sensorimotor synchronization using isochronous (non-fractal) stimuli show that participants' errors exhibit persistent structure (positive long-term correlations), while their inter-tap intervals (ITIs) exhibit anti-persistent structure (negative long-term correlations). Auditory-motor synchronization has not been (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 972